How to cause motion sickness: Let's hope the folks at Guantanamo don't get their hands on one of these

What in the world is this thing?

i-be0b30d02bdd1eb219d6b62a863fe288-optokinetic-drum.jpg

i-eca0cf2af9fc3ac4445c7dff7d8aab70-research.gifIt's called an optokinetic drum, and it's one of the many implements of torture you'll find in a spatial orientation lab. In an optokinetic drum, you sit or stand inside while the entire drum rotates around you. By changing the pattern on the inside of the drum, or by changing the way the drum rotates, a researcher can easily make you lose your lunch, breakfast, or even last night's dinner.

I got the picture of the drum from the Ashton Graybill Spatial Orientation Lab, where you'll find plenty of other devices that I sincerely hope the U.S. military doesn't know about.

Optokinetic drums can be used for practical things, too, like studying animation, or motion sickness. Researchers have found that as long as the drum rotates on a vertical axis and you sit still precisely in the center of a rotating drum painted with vertical stripes, you typically won't get sick. Tilt the drum or the victim a bit to one side, and sickness will ensue quite rapidly. So what does cause motion sickness? A team led by Frederick Bonato has developed a simple experiment to try to narrow down the possibilities.

One theory about motion sickness suggests that you'll get sick any time two sensory inputs about motion conflict: when your eyes detect different motion than your sense of balance (either through your vestibular system in the inner ear, or through sensations across the rest of your body). But in response to the fact that people don't generally get sick in the vertical drum, a second theory has emerged: that only the vertical sense matters -- only when our sensations about vertical conflict will we get sick.

Bonato's team wanted to test that second theory by developing a paradigm where vertical sensations did not conflict, but participants still got sick. They did it by rotating the drum for thirty seconds in one direction, then thirty seconds in the opposite direction. While the drum changed direction, the participants were asked to close their eyes for 5 seconds, so they wouldn't see any of the wobbling while the machine got up to speed. A second group of participants closed their eyes every 30 seconds while the drum rotated in the same direction, and a control group kept their eyes open the entire time while the drum rotated in the same direction.

Every two minutes, participants were asked to report on several different symptoms of motion sickness: for example, dizziness, nausea, dry mouth, and headache. In addition, they rated their overall feeling of sickness on a scale of 0 to 10 (0 = "I feel fine," and 10 = "I feel awful as if I am about to vomit"). Once this second rating reached 5, the experiment was stopped. After 6 minutes in the changing-direction condition, a quarter of participants had dropped out, so the researchers only used the 2- and 4-minute data points. Here are their results:

i-b040e158b2f4cf7431638960f2d1c230-optokinetic.gif

Participants in the changing-direction condition experienced significantly more nausea than the group who saw the drum rotating the same direction every time. A composite score of all motion sickness symptoms showed the same result. Bonato's team argues this shows that motion sickness can occur regardless of whether the sensory conflict is in the vertical direction. In this study, the only difference between the two groups was the horizontal motion of they cylinder, and motion sickness still occurred.

Why don't people sitting in a steadily rotating drum get sick? The team suggests that the key is when a person first opens their eyes inside the drum. The expectation is to see no motion, but the motion of the drum is seen. After a while, the participant gets used to the motion, so there is less of a conflict. But when the motion reverses every 30 seconds, a new conflict between expected and actual motion occurs. Over time, this leads to sickness.

Bonato, F., Bubka, A., & Story, M. (2005). Rotation direction change hastens motion sickness onset in an optokinetic drum. Aviation, Space, and Environmental Medicine, 76(9), 823-826.

More like this

Perhaps we've evolved to interpret certain inconsistencies between our senses and our expectations as evidence of poisoning, in rather the same way that we become sick if we see other people becoming sick -- there may be nothing wrong with us, but our bodies don't see the point in taking chances.

By Caledonian (not verified) on 11 Aug 2006 #permalink

As a victim of one of these rotating drums, I can vouch for their effectiveness at inducing lots and lots of motion sickness.

And, you probably will not be surprised to find out that the US Military does, indeed, know about these things. The Army Research Lab in Aberdeen, MD, is studying the effects of motion sickness on cognitive tasks (or they had planned to, anyway). You can imagine that soldiers are in situations where they get motion sickness all the time.

Yeah, but how do you PREVENT motion sickness-- that's what I want to know!

(from one who can even look down in the car for a few seconds without feeling sick)

Renee - Motion sickness is often linked to poorly developed vestibular / balance system. This is why young children, who's balance is still developing, are more prone to motion sickness.

Motion sickness can be a symptom of dyslexia or similar educational problems. There is some good science linking an immature vestibular system and cerebellum to dyslexia and ADHD.

You can test your balance quite easily. Get a friend to note how much you wobble when you stand on one leg for twenty seconds. Then do it again but with your eyes closed. This takes away the information the balance system was getting visually, forcing it to rely on the vestibular (inner ear). Most people wobble a bit with their eyes closed but the weaker the vestibular system the great difference between the eyes open and eyes closed condition.

If you do have a weak vestibular system then you can train it up by standing on one leg or using a wobble board.

Chris

So, Chris, did you mean that our vestibular system can be improved by training?

Edith - Its not so much improving the vestibular as training the brain to rely on it more. Here's a study on elderly people under going balance training using Tai Chi:

http://www.ms-se.com/pt/re/msse/abstract.00005768-200404000-00014.htm;j…

I under went a 15 month cerebellum / vestibular training programme for my dyslexia. It was then known as DDAT and is now called Dore Achievement Centres. As part of this training you are assessed every 6 weeks using a posturegraph that analyses your balance. All my results are online here:

http://www.myomancy.com/2006/01/dore_ddat_postu.html#more so

can see how my balance changes over time.

Chris