Confirming Aesop - rooks use stones to raise the level of water in a pitcher

Blogging on Peer-Reviewed ResearchAesop's fable "The Crow and the Pitcher" has been confirmed in a wonderful experiment. In the classic tale, a thirsty crow uses stones to raise the level of water in a pitcher until it rises within reach of its beak. This is no mere fiction - rooks, close relatives of crows, have the brains to actually do this.

i-282b365ac62e03dc856d82be5e28666c-Rook.jpgThe aptly named Chris Bird, along with Nathan Emery, gave four captive rooks (Cook, Fry, Connelly and Monroe) a chance to reach a small worm floating in a cylinder of water, with nothing but a small pile of stones sitting on the side. All of them solved the task, and Cook and Fry succeeded on their first attempt. They were savvy about the stones too, using exactly the right number to bring the water within reach and preferring larger stones over smaller ones.

The accomplishments of Bird's rooks are even more impressive when you consider that rooks are not natural tool-users. Many of the corvids - crows, ravens and the like - are avid tool-users and the skills of the New Caledonian crow are rapidly becoming the stuff of popular science legend. But rooks are different - even though they too excel in laboratory tests (as Bird and Emery have previously shown), they hardly ever use tools in the wild. Rather than any special tool-using adaptations, their skills must stem from the sort of general intelligence that great apes are thought to possess.

More so than other members of the family, rooks are extremely opportunistic feeders, relying on a varied menu of seeds, insects, dead meat and rubbish. With such catholic tastes, food is never far from their beaks and they may have little need for specialised tricks involving tools. The same might be true for capuchin monkeys, which happily brandish tools in a lab but only ever use them in the wild when food is scarce.  As Aesop's fable moralised, "Necessity is the mother of invention."

By varying the starting height of the water, Bird and Emery found that the rooks used the stones in a very precise way, putting in the exact number needed to raise the water to a reachable height. They always examined the tube from different angles before they started, and they waited until they had put a few stones in before having a go at reaching the worm. The number of stones they added before making this first attempt was strongly related to the actual number of stones they eventually needed.

Bird and Emery think that the rooks started off with a plan, translating their initial assessment of the water into an estimate of how many stones they'd need. The true test of that hypothesis would be to cover the cylinder after the rooks first see it so that they can't get any feedback on their progress as they lob stones in. Nonetheless, the fact that the rooks only made a grab for the worm after they'd dropped a few stones, even from the first trial, does suggest that they had a specific goal in mind.

As they got the hang of the task, the rooks also moved towards using larger stones that would displace more water. The fact that they all started off with smaller stones suggests that they didn't initially understand the link between size and water displacement, but they quickly learned that the biggest stones were the most effective. Likewise, even though the rooks tried the same technique with a cylinder full of sawdust, they quickly learned that it wouldn't work for this particular medium.

Bird and Emery's results provide yet more evidence of the incredible problem-solving abilities of the crow family, abilities that Aesop documented over two thousand years ago. The same talents allow them to manufacture their own tools, combine different tools into one, and even to crack unyielding nuts by placing them in front of cars stopped at a red light.

The four rooks have starred in past experiments, but never quite like this. They have seen plastic vertical cylinders, but not those containing water. They had used stones as tools, but to collapse a rickety platform rather than to raise a worm into reach. They had probably seen stones in water, but never had to actually do anything with them.

However, Bird and Emery suggest that corvids in general are probably intelligent enough to understand the useful properties of water. In past experiments, various species have used a cup to carry water to dry food, used paper as a sponge, or used a plug to create a pool of water.

Reference: Current Biology 10.1016/j.cub.2009.07.033

Image: Video by Bird and Emery; Rook by R.komorowski

More on crows and intelligent birds:

i-77217d2c5311c2be408065c3c076b83e-Twitter.jpg i-3a7f588680ea1320f197adb2d285d99f-RSS.jpg

More like this

NOT so long ago, the idea that birds might possess some form of what we call intelligence seemed quite ridiculous. Yet this view has changed dramatically in recent years, with numerous studies showing that some bird species are capable of complex cognition. Members of one family of birds in…
You don't have to be particularly intelligent to use tools - many animals do so, including some insects. But it takes a uniquely intelligent animal to be able to combine different tools to solve a problem. We can do it, the great apes can do it, and now the New Caledonian crow joins our exclusive…
There are 19 new articles in PLoS ONE today. As always, you should rate the articles, post notes and comments and send trackbacks when you blog about the papers. You can now also easily place articles on various social services (CiteULike, Mendeley, Connotea, Stumbleupon, Facebook and Digg) with…
New Caledonian crows (Corvus moneduloides) have remarkable tool-using abilities that are at least as sophisticated as those of chimpanzees, if not more so. To date, however, such behaviours have only been observed in contrived experimental conditions. Using newly-developed miniuatrized animal-…

Which behaviors are you expecting corvids to fill in for? Blog posting, sure.

By Nathan Myers (not verified) on 06 Aug 2009 #permalink

Ah, their blog posts would all be the same: "Nevermore, nevermore, nevermore, nevermore..."

This is one of those bits of research that we should be telling kids about so they study science. Hell, this should be on the evening news.

I love science, especially when it does things like this.

At the Lady Bird Johnson Redwood Grove there are signs accusing Steller's Jays of being (along with other corvids) "intelligent, opportunistic, and always looking out for an easy meal." It's a fair cop, I suppose, but who among us looks out for a hard meal?..

Clever birds!
That should make a short on NOVA.

I have to wonder if the pebbles were the only tools provided?
Would they have tried wood chips or some other detritus.
And no, I can't view the vid. My PC is too ancient for most YouTubes.

I never knew that was where the "Neccessity is the mother of invention." quote came from. Obviously I'm lacking in the classics.

It's only a matter of time before the animals outsmart us and kill us all. I'm going vegitarian now to see if that will buy me a quick death when it happens.

By piratebrido (not verified) on 07 Aug 2009 #permalink

This is the earlier experiment done with these same rooks, by Bird and Emery ("Insightful problem solving and creative tool modification by captive nontool-using rooks", PNAS, 2009):

http://www.pnas.org/content/106/25/10370.abstract

The article itself is not open access, but videos of the experiments can be downloaded with no paywall, and they are pretty nifty. There are more obvious controls, too: sometimes a stick is necessary to get the morsel; sometimes a pebble; sometimes a pebble of a particular weight and shape -- and the rooks choose the appropriate one from a selection.

http://www.pnas.org/content/106/25/10370/suppl/DCSupplemental

By Owlmirror (not verified) on 07 Aug 2009 #permalink

Today I saw a crow with a game leg land in a parking lot. He looked back at me, I think, because he seemed not to notice a car coming up behind him. So I gave a quick gesture for him to get out of its path, and he took off quickly.

I don't *know* that I communicated with him. He may have been distracted by something else, and may have heard the car at last just after I gestured. But corvids are so clever, it was worth trying.

By Samantha Vimes (not verified) on 08 Aug 2009 #permalink

One thing that puzzles me is why the experimenter provides the water first and then waits for some time before coming back to provide the stones. What's the point of that?

One speculation is that the bird might have learned to associate "things provided by humans" with "useful things", in which case the point might be to ensure that the bird realises that the stones are meant to be part of the puzzle. But it would be more satisfying (and more Aesopian) to see the bird use stones that are just lying around, not stones that have the metaphorical equivalent of a neon sign saying, "Use These".

How about crows showing altruism too? Across the road from my house is a walnut tree next to a lampost. When the walnuts are ripe the crows pick them off the ground, fly up to the top of the lampost and drop the nuts to crack on the roadway. Also living nearby is a pair of magpies, who don't seem to get the trick, but who hop around below trying to pinch the walnuts. The crows are wise to this and keep the magpies at bay. However, on many occasions I have seen crows who have chased the magpies away many times in succession, fly up with one last nut, drop it, then sit there and watch as the magpie picks it up.

By Phill Marston (not verified) on 08 Aug 2009 #permalink