The Stroop Effect: Score one for object-based attention

i-eca0cf2af9fc3ac4445c7dff7d8aab70-research.gifOne of the amazing things about the Stroop Effect is how much good research is being done based on this simple phenomenon, over 70 years later. One of the neatest recent experiments was created by Peter Wühr and Florian Waszak. I think I've created a simple animation that replicates their results. Click on the image below to bring up a short animated GIF. You'll see an image flash quickly, followed by a blank screen. As quickly as possible after the image flashes, say the color of the rectangle in front. Ignore any words printed on the rectangles; you just want to name the color of the rectangle in front. In case you don't get the idea right away (the images flash pretty quickly), the animation will repeat itself and you'll have a chance to try again.

i-09637ffca8188bb2eb1d6a29fbd71fe6-stroopobject2.gif

So, which part was easier, Part 1 or Part 2? You can give your answer in the poll, below the fold.

The basic Stroop Effect involves trying to name the color a word is printed in instead of reading a word. If the word itself is a color, then when the color the word is printed in is different than the color spelled out by the letters, we're slower name the color (we've made a quick demo of the effect here).

More recently, researchers have found that even if the task is to name the color of a shape, people are still slower when words naming different colors are printed nearby (for example, the word "red" next to a blue square). Amazingly, as long as they are scaled up in size to compensate for poorer peripheral vision, it doesn't matter how far away the words are printed -- the Stroop Effect still occurs.

What we're dealing with, then, is the issue of space- versus object-based attention. If the space between objects doesn't remove the Stroop Effect, then perhaps locating the distracting words on different objects will. That's what the animation above was designed to demonstrate: in Part 1, the distracting words were printed on the object you were attempting to describe; in Part 2, they were the same distance away, but printed on a different object. If object-based attention affects the Stroop Effect, then Part 2 should have been easier than Part 1.

Wühr and Waszak measured reaction times in a similar task much more precisely. A computer timed the precise moment when participants named the color of the rectangle, and the process was repeated over 288 trials. In their experiment, half the time the word matched the color of the rectangle, and half the time it did not. Here are the results:

i-0d7b3d5700ed32931f06e11b9de80793-stroopobject3.gif

The blue line shows the difference in reaction times when the word did not match the rectangle's color. As you can see, when the word was on the relevant object -- the one participants were asked to name -- reaction times were slower than when the word was on the irrelevant object. This was true even though the words were the same distance away from the fixation point at the center of the screen. The yellow line shows reaction times when the color and the word matched. As you can see, the reactions are much faster. When the word was printed on the irrelevant object, there was still a Stroop Effect: the non-matching words still had a slower reaction time, but the difference between the two reaction times was significantly smaller.

Two additional experiments, where the participants were asked to identify the color of the rectangle in back, and when distractor words were printed on the background instead of one of the rectangles, confirmed the result.

Wühr and Waszak conclude that object-based attention plays an important role in the Stroop task, possibly more important than that of space-based attention.

So, did our little demonstration work for you? Could you detect the effects of object-based attention without the benefit of measuring reaction time? Let us know about your experiences with this task in the comments.

Wühr, P., & Waszak, F. (2003). Object-based attentional selection can modulate the Stroop Effect. Memory & Cognition, 31(6), 983-994.

More like this

I'm sure most Cognitive Daily readers are aware of the massive debate permeating the scientific world these days. No, not evolution versus creationism; I'm talking about object- versus space-based attention. Haven't heard of this raging debate? Well, then, let me refer you to a fascinating pair of…
The Stroop Effect was originally just a language effect: we're slower identifying the color text is printed in when the words themselves name different colors. In the 81 years since the effect was first observed, it's been applied to a variety of very different phenomena. In general, the effect is…
When two athletes are the same size and strength, what makes one better than the other? In many sports, the best athletes are the ones who can react more quickly to game situations than others. Are they just generally better at focusing their attention where it needs to be? Or have they learned…
It's impossible to pay attention to everything in the visual field at once. If we could, magicians would be out of business: most "magic" tricks work by distracting the viewers' attention while the real trick is being done in plain sight. However, if a new object enters our field of view, we…

in Part 1, the distracting words were printed on the object you were attempting to describe; in Part 2, they were the same distance away, but printed on a different object. If object-based attention affects the Stroop Effect, then Part 2 should have been more difficult than Part 1.

Should that read "part 2 should have been easier than part 1?"

Definitely was for me. I buy it.

By ThePolynomial (not verified) on 29 Jun 2006 #permalink

I'm used to doing tasks which require strict (and measured) central fixation, so my natural inclination is to foveate at the center of things. Since the color to be named was always present in the center, I could accomplish either task by processing only that information -- narrowing my "spotlight" if you will. Consequently it seemed to me that each part was equally easy/difficult. I'm not really sure what words were popping up in the periphery, regardless of whether they appeared at the horizontal or vertical meridian...

I also focused my attention in the very center, so I didn't even notice the text. The center is what tells me which object is in front, so there is no point in looking at the edges.

The Polynomial: "Should that read "part 2 should have been easier than part 1?""

Oops! Yes -- fixed it.

Corby/Eso: actually, in the original experiment, participants were instructed to fixate on the center of the display, and a cross was placed there for them to fixate upon; the effect was still there. It's possible that without the fixation point the effect is more dramatic, which might be why you didn't experience the effect.

Hmm, I can't use that poll, because for me the first time I saw either, they were both easy to tell the color. But the *second* round (and the next), the first was harder and the second easier, probably because the images and patters got established.

What does this say to anyone? The effect is easily understandable and the probability that it is object-based is not all that confounding. So what are the implications for instructional design? If you are employing distractors for some reason, be aware that sharing an object may count for more than proximity does? Okaaay ...

By tom nickel (not verified) on 29 Jun 2006 #permalink

Part 1 and Part 2 were basicly the same for me (taking into account the learning curve). But why? Though I love to read, I suffer from reading problems, which have always made me a really slow reader. Plus, I am 70 and in need of new glasses. So, of course, I reacted first and foremost to the color itself. Hmmm, is this a clue as to why I am so often confused by signage and by written instruction?