What backpack-wearing toddlers can tell us about how kids learn to walk

ResearchBlogging.orgJim was an early, confident walker. Greta likes to say that he didn't learn to walk, he went straight to running. By the time he was about 16 months old, he could already outrun his already-pregnant mother.

i-03795f6e834c68293de18a771966a074-garciguirre1.jpg

Nora, on the other hand, was a late, tentative walker. She took her first steps at around 12 months, and still wasn't very confident as a walker at 18 months. In this photo, at 17 months, she still clings to their toy kitchen set for balance.

i-ab512af4e72362741b3f7179164bd15c-garciguirre2.jpg

But I've just finished reading a fascinating study suggesting that at 14 months, when both of them were walking -- Jim with confidence, and Nora struggling -- they actually took a similar approach to balance while walking.

A team led by Jessie Garciaguirre might be the first to investigate how infants who've only recently learned to walk adapt to carrying heavy loads. Adults generally carry no more than 35 percent of their own body weight (though in some African tribes, women balance immense loads -- up to 70 percent of their body weight -- on their heads). School-age kids might port 20-30 percent of body weight in backpacks on their way to school. Adults and kids make significant adjustments to posture and gait when bearing loads in excess of 15 percent of body weight. They take shorter steps, and they lean away from the load to compensate (generally this means leaning forward to accommodate a backpack).

So why not put backpacks on toddlers and see how they manage? Garciaguirre and Karen Adolph had previously found that 14-month-olds fall down an average of 15 times per hour while playing. What could possibly go wrong when heavy weights are strapped to their backs?

Actually the infants were carefully monitored and "spotted" by an experimenter during the study to ensure that no one got hurt, and this research can offer tremendous insight into how children learn to walk. The team recruited 27 14-month-olds and fitted them with a vest that could accommodate weights in four positions:

i-b5eac9f65f15f44336a4dbc86ca77b38-garciguirre3.gif

The toddlers then walked along a raised, 3.66-meter walkway that sensed their footsteps while they were videotaped from two different angles. Each child made the walk several times: with no weights, with weights in front, in back, evenly distributed, or on the right or left sides. The weights were customized for each child to ensure a consistent 15 percent of body weight.

Then an observer rated each video to indicate whether the child was leaning forward, backward, or to the side. Do toddlers lean in the same way as adults, to counterbalance the weights they carry? Here are the results:

i-b87b163d54c159f0a7dc343f914d34bf-garciguirre4.gif

When the weights are placed on toddler's backs, instead of leaning forward to compensate, the vast majority actually leaned back! When the weights were in front, they leaned forward. Needless to say, this made walking quite difficult. When the weights were in back, there were an average of 2.69 disruptions in walking gait over the short 3.66 meter walkway. In fact, only 10 of the 54 trials with weights on the back were completed without disruption. With no weights, 47 of the 54 trials were disruption-free.

Infants actually had a harder time with weights on their back than any other condition, including the front or left or right side (this isn't to say those weight positions were disruption-free: half or more of those trials were disrupted as well). So why is walking with weights on the back so difficult for toddlers? Garciaguirre's team says it's probably because it's more difficult for anyone to walk while leaning back than leaning forward. Since our feet extend forward, we can lean farther forward than backward.

These toddlers, some with over four months of walking experience, simply haven't yet learned to lean away from a weight to balance it. The amount of walking experience each child had bore no relationship to their walking behavior; the same trends were seen in all the children.

That said, all the toddlers did change their gaits in response to the weights. They took shorter steps, spent more time with both feet on the ground, and they slowed down overall, just as adults do when carrying heavy weights. The failure to lean to balance the weight appeared to account for all the differences between weight-bearing for toddlers and adults.

Related: How we learn to walk.

Garciaguirre, J.S., Adolph, K.E., Shrout, P.E. (2007). Baby Carriage: Infants Walking With Loads. Child Development, 78(2), 664-680. DOI: 10.1111/j.1467-8624.2007.01020.x

More like this

Learning to walk was a passion for my son Jimmy. He would sweat and struggle with it until finally he had it mastered—and then it was off to the races. My daughter Nora, by contrast, didn't seem to mind not being able to walk. After all, if you didn't walk, then some sweet grown-up would soon show…
I'm usually disappointed when I try to take a picture of a steep precipice—it never seems as impressive in the photo as it did when I was standing right there. Take this photo, for example. It's a nice shot of my daughter Nora, taken on our hike in the Great Smoky Mountains this past summer, but…
One of Jimmy's favorite toys as a toddler was a simple little bucket of blocks. There were three shapes: a rectangular prism, a triangular prism, and a cylinder. The bucket's lid had three holes: a square, a triangle, and a circle (The picture at right was the only one I could find online -- this…
The NYTimes ran an excerpt of a book called Rethinking Thin: The New Science of Weight Loss -- and the Myths and Realities of Dieting by Gina Kolata. Having read the excerpt -- I haven't read the whole book -- I take issue with how Kolata frames the issue of the genetics of obesity. My problem…

Take a look at this!

Oh, but to see the videos from this study! I appreciate the questions the researchers are asking about how children learn, but I keep giggling thinking about how the children must look! It had to be darling . . .

:)

By ctenotrish (not verified) on 14 May 2008 #permalink

These toddlers, some with over four months of walking experience, simply haven't yet learned to lean away from a weight to balance it. The amount of walking experience each child had bore no relationship to their walking behavior; the same trends were seen in all the children.

Unless 'walking experience' is taken to mean experience with walking that includes learning to handle significant weights while walking prior to 14 months, it is likely a genetically programmed walking sequence explains the study results. In general, the sequence involves the transition from a somewhat flat-footed, penguin-like walking gait during infancy, to the familiar heel-toe pattern from age 5 onward. Greater difficulty with weights on the back and leaning backward rather than forward when weight is placed on the back could be explained from a biomechanical developmental perspective.

If one stands up straight and then leans backwards, the knees should flex somewhat (and likely buckle) with significant weight. If one leans forward, the knees will extended and should not buckle with the same weight. Weights on the back are likely to cause more trouble because it is essentially the difference between bearing significant weight when the knees are bent vs. when they are straight. 14-month-olds probably don't lean forward when weights are placed on the back because they have not yet developed the heel-toe walking pattern, which involves two knee extensions (one during the heel plant and another during the push off with the toes). They are walking more flat-footed and are likely to have more knee flexion, which again, is a position less conducive to weight bearing, especially on the back. Adults might automatically lean forward with weight on the back because they have developed the heel-toe pattern, and it is the toes that ultimately propel the walker forward. Leaning back the way children do puts more body weight on the heels and would make the heel-toe action more difficult.

Another interesting study would be a repeat of this one, but using infants around 18 months. At around this age, children tend to show the developmental milestone of walking on the toes, which is probably an important step (pun intended) towards developing heel-toe action. It's likely that they will begin to show the adult pattern of leaning forward to bear weight with the development of the tip-toe walking milestone.

By Tony Jeremiah (not verified) on 14 May 2008 #permalink

Garciaguirre and Karen Adolph had previously found that 14-month-olds fall down an average of 15 times per hour while playing. What could possibly go wrong when heavy weights are strapped to their backs?

Hehe. Quote of the week right there