Joint Attention and Social Compentence, or what a baby pointing at a toy says about well-behaved toddlers

ResearchBlogging.orgOne of the key components of "normal" child development is social competence. We expect kids to become gradually better at behaving respectfully towards peers, to comply with requests made by others, to understand the thoughts of others, to play together with kids and adults, to sustain attention, and to be motivated to learn. But what makes the difference between a child who becomes socially competent and one who doesn't? Obviously there are some risk factors, such as whether they have autism, whether both parents are present in the household, and the education and poverty level of the family. But some kids who seem to have all the advantages still have trouble getting along with others. Why?

Some studies have found that at-risk babies show some early warning signs that are associated with later poor social competence. It's possible, for example, to measure several dimensions of "joint attention." Take a look at this old picture of Jim and Nora playing with their kitchen set:

i-093133343d87975846244dc98b2c155e-Hecke1.jpg

Aside from the fact that they're absolutely adorable, you can see that Jim is reaching for some utensils and Nora is following his reach and looking at the same thing. This is an example of Nora responding to joint attention. (I should add that it's not the best example because the classic case would have Jim pointing, not touching an object -- but it's the best I could find right now, flying cross-country at 30,000 feet.)

From Jim's perspective, he's initiating joint attention -- directing Nora's attention to an object he's interested in. (Again, not the best example of this since it's not clear Jim wants to direct Nora's attention to the object.) A third type of joint attention is initiating behavior requests, such as when an infant points to an object out of her reach in order to "ask" an adult to get it for her.

You might think that these different types of joint attention are all just manifestations of the the same phenomenon, but studies of at-risk children have found that different aspects of joint attention are associated with later social competence in different ways -- which brings us back to our original question. Do typically developing kids also show the same warning signs in infancy?

Amy Vaughan Van Hecke and eight other researchers tracked 52 children from age 12 months until they were 30 months old. Initially the infants sat at a table on their parent's lap. An experimenter across the table had a basket of toys. The experimenter spent 20 minutes systematically playing with the toys and pointing at objects in different parts of the room in ways that were designed to provide opportunities for the baby to demonstrate each type of joint attention.

The researchers then contacted tested each child again with different measures at 15 months old, 24 months old, and 30 months old. Their results matched the earlier studies of at-risk infants: there was no general relationship between joint attention and later communication skills or social competence. Instead, different types of joint attention predicted different results at different ages. For example, babies who had exceptionally high ability to initiate behavior requests at 12 months were more likely to be difficult to soothe at 15 months, but also more likely to understand more words at 24 months. Initiating behavior requests had no significant correlation with social competence at 30 months. But those who had high-level ability to initiate joint attention at 12 months, like Jim in the picture, were likely to be better able to express themselves in language at 24 months (but not comprehend more words). And this ability was the only joint attention skill that correlated significantly with social competence at 30 months.

So while there's a clear relationship between some joint attention skills and social competence, it's also clear that some joint attention skills are better than others. What this study doesn't show is what causes joint attention skills themselves. Are we born with these skills, or do we learn them in early infancy?

It's also important to note that even high-level initiation of joint attention at 12 months isn't a perfect predictor of social competence at 30 months. Many babies who aren't pointing to things at 12 months still end up being socially competent.

Amy Vaughan Van Hecke, Peter C. Mundy, C. Françoise. Acra, Jessica J. Block, Christine E. F. Delgado, Meaghan V. Parlade, Jessica A. Meyer, A. Rebecca Neal, Yuly B. Pomares (2007). Infant Joint Attention, Temperament, and Social Competence in Preschool Children Child Development, 78 (1), 53-69 DOI: 10.1111/j.1467-8624.2007.00985.x

More like this

When Jim was about 13 months old, I happened to be enrolled in a graduate level developmental psychology class. Our big term paper assignment involved observing two children at different developmental stages. I decided it would be cool to do a "longitudinal study" of Jim's language development…
Baby sign language is all the rage these days. Upscale day-care centers and nanny services promote it as a better way of understanding what babies want. Babies have been known to reliably produce signs as young as 5.5 months, and studies have shown that they reliably produce signs significantly…
Nora was an excellent talker, starting at a very young age, but that didn't mean that she couldn't express herself in other ways. Here, for example, she points to a the item she wants. It's entirely possible that she didn't yet know the word "stick," but she was still quite able to express her…
One of things I was taught over and over again when I was in education school was the importance of getting parents involved in kids' learning. If you get the parents on your side, my professors insisted, then you're going to be much more able to get through to the students. I didn't last long…

When I was young, we had a dog.
I'd point at things for it to go and fetch, or I'd point at directions I wanted it to go in.
It would simply look at my finger.

I wonder if humans, and our 'special' 'speech gene' (or linguistic parasite that alters our behaviour to its own ends) first intentionally builds the linguistic spacial metaphor language of gestures that acts as the primitive or OS-level layer upon which language can then be cultured or installed or gotten?

...babies who had exceptionally high ability to initiate behavior requests at 12 months were more likely to be difficult to soothe at 15 months, but also more likely to understand more words at 24 months. Initiating behavior requests had no significant correlation with social competence at 30 months. But those who had high-level ability to initiate joint attention at 12 months, like Jim in the picture, were likely to be better able to express themselves in language at 24 months (but not comprehend more words). And this ability was the only joint attention skill that correlated significantly with social competence at 30 months.

Sounds like this may be connected to language learning styles (referential and expressive) that are evidenced by two years. Toddlers with a referential language style primarily use words that reference objects (e.g., mommy shoe, go car, more cookie); toddlers with an expressive language style use more pronouns and reference social situations (e.g., thank you, stop it, i want). Toddlers with a referential style believe that words are for naming things; those with an expressive style believe that words are for talking about people's feelings and needs.

It's possible that the more difficult to soothe babies with greater speech comprehension have a referential style, which implicitly involves a one-way communicative style (i.e., the purpose of communication involves acquiring things for self). Alternatively, those showing more joint attention initiation and greater speech production later on, probably have an expressive style that implicitly involves two-way communication (i.e., the purpose of communication is to share one's own thoughts and feelings with others). This seems to explain the data showing a link between greater speech production and social competence in those displaying more joint attention initiation.

Furthermore, the hypothetical link between expressive style and joint attention seems connected to an earlier article on gesture and theory of mind, which shows that gesturing (which is what joint attention directly involves) is an important precursor to developing a theory of mind (which itself is an important cognitive ability involving the ability to understand that other people have thoughts separate from our own).

By Tony Jeremiah (not verified) on 28 Jul 2008 #permalink

@Ian - It could just be your dog, since in our house we communicate with our dog primarily by pointing, after getting his attention with his name.

It could just be Ian's dog AND my dogs AND my kids. :) My dogs, like Ian's, just look at my finger when I point. There are times when my kids do too, and I have to say, "I'm POINTING; look where my finger is POINTING." And these are bright kids!

By --Lisa S. (not verified) on 28 Jul 2008 #permalink

It would be an interesting follow-up to measure joint attention for children with different numbers of siblings. Since it seems it takes at least two for someone to initiate joint attention (like in your photo), then would only-childs have lower joint attention because of a smaller audience? Also, how about your order of birth? Would older children command more attention from their younger siblings? (also like your photo)

That's not really a fair test for a dog, because that is not what we've bred domestic dogs for. Wolves tend to spend a lot more time looking around at the things around them in the environment; domestic dogs, on the other hand, have been bred and conditioned to look instead at human faces, to recieve instructions. See, for example, http://www.mtapi.hu/userdirs/33/Publikaciok_Topal_J/MiklosietalCB2003.p… .

By Luna_the_cat (not verified) on 30 Jul 2008 #permalink

My children love peppa pig toys - the characters are well created with loveable, cheeky little piggy who lives with her little brother George, Mummy Pig and Daddy Pig. Peppa Pig adventures appeal to childrens imagination and inventive play.