Polygamous Males Have Larger...Hippocampi!

ResearchBlogging.orgIt should not come as a surprise to the regular reader of this blog that a lot can be learned about animal cognition by simply observing animal behavior. But can observing animal behavior lead the observer to make inferences about brain anatomy? Can observing animal behavior tell us something about the evolution of the brain?

i-9c3117b5fa5abb5286db8adf98c981d9-racoon-yes.jpg

Figure 1: Like the raccoon says.

Let's say you have two very very closely related species. You might even call them congeneric, because they are from the same taxonomic genus. In most ways, these two species are very similar, but they differ behaviorally in some very big ways. Might those behavioral differences predict neurobiological differences?

The different species of the genus microtus display the full range of mammalian mating systems: some species are entirely monogamous, with two individuals mating for life, while other species are fully polygamous, with males having multiple female mates.

i-864012066d39893d97ea3f6a2da1a302-Pine Vole.jpg

Figure 2: Meet the monogamous pine vole (microtus pinetorum).

i-8bb3566d4c65a725096b33864f0acb59-Meadow Vole.jpg

Figure 3: Meet the polygamous meadow vole (microtus pennsylvanicus).

The difference in mating style leads to a very important difference in spatial memory requirements: for monogamous species, males and females both tend to live their lives in areas of land roughly the same size. For polygamous species, however, males' home ranges are much larger in spatial extent than the home range of the females (as well as the typical home ranges of the monogamous male voles). This makes sense: the polygamous male's home range needs to include the smaller home ranges of multiple female mates.

mono area.jpgi-79928dbf09b06d47fc18e58590a5c14b-polyarea-thumb-250x130-50203.jpg

Figure 4: On the left, a schematic diagram for the home ranges of monogamous voles. On the right, a schematic for the home ranges of polygamous voles. Solid pink blobs for females, dotted red lines for males.

Importantly, these behavioral differences in the polygamous voles are only seen in mature adults during mating season. Juvenile males voles of this species do not have significantly larger home ranges compared to females, and adults do not have larger home ranges during the rest of the year. These species and sex related differences probably create an important selective pressure for spatial memory.

The first question to ask is whether or not the observed sex and species differences in the spatial extent of the home range results in differences in spatial memory, as you might hypothesize. And the basic answer is yes: under laboratory conditions, voles of polygamous species show strong gender differences in tests of spatial ability, with males outperforming females. Voles of monogamous species fail to show these sex differences under identical testing conditions.

If the sexual selection for ranging behavior has influenced the evolution of cognition (at least with respect to spatial knowledge), then it is possible that it has also influenced the evolution of the parts of the brain known to subserve spatial navigation or spatial memory.

The hippocampus is known to play an important role in spatial learning. Rodents with hippocampal lesions show impaired performance on spatial tasks. In addition, the relative size of the hippocampus is larger in birds whose food-caching locations are located within a larger area, compared with other birds who use different food-caching strategies.

To test the hypothetical relationship between ranging behavior and hippocampal volume, the researchers went out and captured 40 wild voles during breeding season - 10 male and 10 female pine voles, and 10 male and 10 female meadow voles - and compared the size of the hippocampus between sexes for each species. (It would not make sense to directly compare the species, but it would make sense to compare the sex differences in hippocampus size between species.)

On average, the hippocampi of the polygamous male were 3.2 cubic millimeters larger than those of the females. For the monogamous voles, the males' hippocampi were only 0.5 cubic millimeters larger than the females. This aligns with the hypothesis, but comparing absolute differences between species and sexes isn't the proper comparison, since there are sex and species differences in total brain volume. When analyzing the ratio of hippocampal volume to total brain volume across sex and species, the differences remain statistically significant. The sex difference observed in the hippocampus volume in polygamous voles was significantly larger than that observed for the monogamous voles.

i-04b2ac0febd986d4ef1a57d65ffa9712-voleshippocampus.jpg

Figure 5: Left, absolute values for hippocampal volume. Right, controlled for total brain volume. No difference for monogamous pine voles, but males hippocampi are significantly larger in polygamous meadow voles.

One principle of brain evolution that is intuitively appealing is that larger neural mass is related to increased information processing. However, it is hard to empirically test this principle, because it is difficult to isolate neural structures and then to assess their functions. However, this was one study that was able to successfully validate this principle (called Jerison's principle of proper mass), at least for one structure (the hippocampus) which has repeatedly been shown to be involved the processing of a specific type of information (spatial processing), in two vole species.

It is important to note that on the basis of these findings, it is not possible to conclusively determine whether or not the observed sex differences are determined solely by sex or if they require experience. Only a controlled laboratory study could address this question. However, the selective pressure created by mating style on spatial abilities, and the subsequent neurobiological differences observed, seem fairly well supported. The authors point out, and rightly so, that the presence of absence of experiential effects does not preclude an evolutionary explanation the observed sex differences.

So, if you learned only one thing today: monogamous males have smaller hippocampi.

If you learned a second thing today: Sexual selection, arising from social ecology, can produce behavioral sex differences via changes in the underlying neurobiological substrate.

Jacobs, L. (1990). Evolution of Spatial Cognition: Sex-Specific Patterns of Spatial Behavior Predict Hippocampal Size Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences, 87 (16), 6349-6352. DOI: 10.1073/pnas.87.16.6349

Categories

More like this

Love demands an explanation. Less than 5% of mammal species live monogamously, with males and females staying together beyond mating, and fathers helping mothers care for babies. We humans aren't the most monogamous species of the bunch, but we're closer to that end of the spectrum than the other…
Variation in neural V1aR predicts sexual fidelity and space use among male prairie voles in semi-natural settings: Although prairie voles (Microtus ochrogaster) are socially monogamous, males vary in both sexual and spatial fidelity. Most males form pairbonds, cohabit with one female, and defend…
People, scientists included, are always looking for simple, comprehensible explanations for complex phenomena. It's so satisfying to be able to easily explain something in a sound bite, and sound bites are so much more easily accepted by an audience than some elaborate, difficult collection of…
Love is all around us and love is in the air, and if I know my mainstream science reporters, today they will have you believe that love is in our genes too. A new report suggests that variation in a gene called AVPR1A has a small but evident influence on the strength of a relationship, the…

Hippocampaus size is not related on their being polygamous but in order to remember directions they should have more memory. It is more related to how they can easily navigate, polygamy is just the advantage because of that ability and not vice-versa.

These species and sex related differences probably create an important selective pressure for spatial memory.

Why does selective pressure come in at all? This could be within the range of natural plasticity -- particularly if the plasticity itself has been selected for over the history of these species and their ancestral species. Why go conceptually digging in the genome when the hippocampi could be growing and shrinking in relationship to activity directly? Doesn't the fact that juvenile males don't have the bonus hippocampus suggest exactly that -- this isn't due to "special" genes leading to differential development, but "normal" genes responding to differential behavior.

Why does everyone go for the "simple genetic explanation" regardless of the evidence in front of their eyes? Yes, everything has a genetic component, but that doesn't mean it's the proximate cause.

@3: Doesn't the fact that juvenile males don't have the bonus hippocampus suggest exactly that -- this isn't due to "special" genes leading to differential development, but "normal" genes responding to differential behavior.

They could be the same genes, or they could be variants of the same genes.

But consider the growth of facial hair in human males. This is certainly innate. No experience is needed for this to emerge. The fact is, however, that it emerges late in life, at puberty. Why couldn't enlarged hippocampi in voles similarly simply emerge later in life, despite being innate? It would be costly, from an evolutionary standpoint, to fuel a larger brain until it was needed, when the voles become sexually mature.

And you no doubt saw the cautionary note:

It is important to note that on the basis of these findings, it is not possible to conclusively determine whether or not the observed sex differences are determined solely by sex or if they require experience. Only a controlled laboratory study could address this question. However, the selective pressure created by mating style on spatial abilities, and the subsequent neurobiological differences observed, seem fairly well supported. The authors point out, and rightly so, that the presence of absence of experiential effects does not preclude an evolutionary explanation [for] the observed sex differences.

So are you saying that monogamous males are hippocampussies?!

Jason,

If I may extend this study to the human species; men who cheat need a larger hippo to remember all the lies they've told in order to continue cheating successfully. Hmmm...

Thanks for sharing.

The fact is, however, that it emerges late in life, at puberty. Why couldn't enlarged hippocampi in voles similarly simply emerge later in life, despite being innate? It would be costly, from an evolutionary standpoint, to fuel a larger brain until it was needed, when the voles become sexually mature.

If you learned a second thing today: Sexual selection, arising from social ecology, can produce behavioral sex differences via changes in the underlying neurobiological substrate.