I've got a long article in Nature this week on Jeff Lichtman (of Brainbow fame) and the birth of connectomics, which seeks to construct a complete wiring diagram of the brain:
At first glance, Jeff Lichtman seems to be hanging long strips of sticky tape from the walls of his Harvard lab. The tape flutters in the breeze from the air-conditioner. But closer inspection reveals that this is not tape: it is the brain of a mouse, rendered into one long, delicate strip of tissue and fixed onto a plastic film. When the film is tilted to the light, the tissue becomes visible, like the smear of a greasy fingerprint.
These smudges are the creation of a new brain-slicing machine invented by Lichtman, a molecular and cellular biologist at Harvard University, along with Kenneth Hayworth, a graduate student at the University of Southern California, Los Angeles. Called the automatic tape-collecting lathe ultramicrotome (ATLUM), the machine resembles an old-fashioned film projector with two large reels. At its centre is a fixed diamond blade that cuts continuously into a rotating mouse brain, much like an apple parer. The end result is a seamless sliver of tissue, less than 10 nanometres thick and around 5 metres long, that is deposited on the plastic film spinning around the spools.
Although Lichtman appreciates the technical precision of the ATLUM -- "That's a real diamond!" he says -- he is most excited about its scientific potential. Researchers in his lab are starting to put these slices under an electron microscope to visualize the intricate web of connections between neighbouring neurons. Lichtman eventually hopes to have a 'farm' of several dozen such microscopes scanning tissue around the clock. Even then it would take months, if not years, to capture all the connections in the strip from a single brain. "When you cut the brain this thin, there's just such a massive amount to see," he says. "It does require us to think about imaging on a different scale."
Lichtman likes to think on a different scale. In recent years, he has become a leading proponent of a new field that is working to create a connectome, a complete map of neural wiring in the mammalian brain. Currently, such a map exists only for the nematode Caenorhabditis elegans, which has 302 neurons. The adult human brain, in contrast, contains 100 billion neurons and several trillion synaptic connections. "I know the goal sounds daunting," Lichtman says. He insists that such a wiring diagram is an essential undertaking, because it will allow scientists to see, for the first time, the path that information takes as it is shuttled from cell to cell, and how all these cells and the information they transmit weave together to create a conscious brain.
I'm especially interested in Lichtman's contention that the standard deductive model of modern science - generate a theory and then find a way to test it - isn't adequate for solving the brain. Instead, he argues that neuroscience needs to return to the inductive approach of Victorian science, so that scientists begin by carefully observing the brain and only then generate testable ideas. (Darwin, for instance, was an inductivist.) The basic idea is that the brain is so complicated an organ that it's nearly impossible to generate decent theories a priori.
- Log in to post comments
I disagree with a renewed emphasis on an inductive approach. If you take a look at chaos theory and epigenetic phenomena, the brain is way too complex to look at things inductively. In such a dynamic system where a single neuron could have 10,000 synapses with other neurons as well as autocommunication(which then also have 10,000 synapses to other neurons), how can you design a decent controlled study around that. I am not saying that an inductive analysis doesn't have its uses, but our whole research system has been precedented on this reductionist thinking and it has led to limitations in our understanding of complex/dynamic phenomena like that which occurs in the brain.
It's like trying to understand the stock market, everything can make sense that a company should go up (or down) based on its fundamentals and the needs of a market, but that still has no bearing on how the market will respond, how the rest of the sector is doing, and so on and so forth. You have to accept the presence of the unknown, the unpredictability, and base new research designs on this inherent nature to give new insights as to how to increase the probability of such and such events occurring, rather than focusing on specific, "intended" changes in one pathway at a time. In the brain and especially neurotransmitter interactions, "right" can be "left", and "left" can be "right" depending on limitless confounding variables. Individual research studies on specific pathways can give insights, but to piece together anything useful from these studies, would be incredibly time-consuming and expensive. A whole is not necessarily the sum of its parts, when those parts can synergize and create dynamic phenomena ESPECIALLY in a biological system where things like signal amplification and positive/negative feedback loops are the norm.
Jonah: Compare your closing paragraph above with this excerpt from your piece in Seed, "Out of the Blue":
I hate to belabor the point, but induction is a myth. Science always proceeds by constructing and testing theories or models. However, in certain domainsâand neuroscience is certainly one of themâthis process must involve intense engagement with empirical data, sometimes massive amounts of it. Still, assumptions are being made all along the way about what data to collect. Good scientists find ways to check these assumptions wherever they can, notwithstanding the fact that there is never any final guarantee that they've checked them well enough.
Hi. Please correct me if I'm wrong, but isn't this just a matter of semantics? Basically, what I get from Lichtman's comments about becoming inductivists again is the message that we need to take a step back, look empirically at how connection patterns are organized, and then start making theories. Isn't this what everyone is saying, though? I'm not getting the real meat of how a top-notch neural inductivist versus a top-notch neural deductivist would practically differ in their modalities and mentalities.
Thank you in advance to anyone who can help illuminate how this is more than semantic.
They'd both go looking for data. The inductivist would formulate a theory about how things are and then look for "proof" ie try to disprove it and fail, while also showing that surprising predictions come true. A deductionist would get the data first and then say, ah, so that's how it works, I suspected as much but I didn't want to taint the experiment with bias by saying so ahead of time.
Nice piece in Nature, dude! The main obstacle to achieving a connectome is the computational one.
Marco,
did you get it backward? or am I really confused?
Beck F