A horse is a horse, of course of course

In general, the ability to attribute attention to others seems important: it allows an animal to notice the presence of other individuals (whether conspecifics, prey, or predators) as well as important locations or events by following the body orientation or eyegaze of others. We've spent a lot of time here at The Thoughtful Animal thinking about how domestication has allowed dogs to occupy a unique niche in the social lives of humans. They readily understand human communication cues such as eye-gaze and finger-pointing, and capitalize on the infant-caregiver attachment system to have their own needs met. There are several explanations for the emergence of these abilities in dogs:
(1) domestic dogs inherited these abilities from their wolf ancestors;
(2) dogs learn social communication cues by associative learning, simply by sharing physical and social space with humans; and
(3) through the process of domestication, the ability to read human social-communicative cues has emerged due to selection pressures and convergent evolution.

There is pretty solid evidence that dogs far outperform wolves in these tasks, so for now we can safely place that explanation to the side. There is evidence that various species, following intense experience with humans, display improved abilities in reading human social cues, including apes, dolphins, seals, ravens, and parrots. However, in dogs this skill emerges incredibly early in development (before there's been the same sort of intense enculturation that other species require), and does not appear to change significantly with age (remember the thing about finding developmental signatures?) That domestication appears to be the best explanation for the emergence of these abilities in dogs suggests that other domesticated species may show similar skills.

Indeed, there are other animals that have been domesticated, and not just as companion animals or pets. The five classic "barnyard" mammals are all considered domesticated: cows, pigs, sheep, goats, and horses. And horses, in particular, operate within a really interesting framework in which they are both considered work animals as well as, in some cases, companion animals. They don't live in our homes, as dogs do, but neither are they bred for production, like pigs, cows, sheep, and goats. Whatever their exact role, it seems clear that they would benefit from the ability to comprehend human social communicative cues.

ResearchBlogging.orgHorses (both domestic and wild) generally communicate visually (though vocal signals also play a role), using body language, and in particular, by small coordinated movements of their heads, ears, and eyes. So, horses have a natural predisposition to detect subtle facial and head-related cues, have been domesticated by humans for several thousands of years, and most individuals are fairly well enculturated. Taken together, this may allow domestic horses to read human communicative gestures particularly well. However, with the exception of the famous case of Clever Hans (which deserves a post of its own), there hasn't been an extensive investigation into the ability of horses to comprehend human social cues - until recently, when several researchers from the Center for Mammal Vocal Communication and Cognition Research at the University of Sussex began investigating horses.

In all, fifty-two horses participated in their experiment, with 36 included in the final analysis, ranging in age from 10 months to 38 years, all living socially in herds, some of whom were occasionally ridden. While the extent of human enculturation was not known for most of the horses, all had at least some exposure to humans.

First, the horses participated in the warm-up phase to introduce them to the general idea and to the experimenters. The subject was held at the release point (R) while the two experimenters approached the horse from the center (C) with their hands out in front of them, so that they could give the horse a food reward. The experimenters then withdrew back to point C, so that the handler could then lead the horse to follow and receive a second food reward. The reward was delivered jointly by the two experimenters (who also rotated positions) so that the horses would not develop a preference for one individual or the other. The idea was to shape a response across 10 trials in which, when released at point R, the horse would approach the experimenters at point C to receive a reward. This was done with attentive experimenters, who faced and made eye contact with the horses, and also with inattentive experimenters, who displayed body and face positions that were (as you might expect) not directed towards the horses.

i-b1d2ce6341bfc7d6f165459a08306944-horse setup.jpg

Figure 1: Experimental set-up.

The test trials included four cued trials (which did not include food rewards), punctuated by reinforcement trials (which included food rewards, to maintain motivation). The experimenters took their positions (E). During each trial, one experimenter was inattentive, and one experimenter was attentive. Experimenters returned to point C, at which time the horses were released from point R to retrieve their reward. Subjects were then led in a figure eight around the test area (either to the right or to the left) before beginning the next trial.

Four different types of cues were tested. The attentive person stood facing forwards and maintained eye contact with the horse as it approached. The inattentive person stood with his back towards the subject, or with his body forwards, but his head turned away, or with body and head forwards, but with eyes closed. During the fourth "mixed" condition (to control for the other three), the attentive person stood with his body turned away from the subjects but his head turned towards the subject to maintain eye contact, while the inattentive person stood with his body forwards but his head turned away.

horse conditions.jpg

Figure 2: Still images of the four conditions.

The key variable, as you might expect, was which experimenter the horse approached. The differences between the conditions would allow the experimenters to determine which - if any - of the attentional cues (body position, head position, eye open/closed) the horses used in determining which experimenter to approach. A choice was considered correct if the horse approached and stood within one meter of the attentive experimenter within sixty seconds of release.

The horses chose to approach the attentive person significantly more often than the inattentive person in the body, head, and eye conditions, but not in the fourth "mixed" condition. Scores were not correlated with age, suggesting that younger horses did as well as the older horses, so that can be considered as evidence that the results are not due to intensive enculturation - that there is possibly a genetic component, or that horses are born prepared to quickly learn these cues. Further studies with foals (infant horses) could help to clarify this issue.

i-18cef8cd6a35346702ad67bf4d8851c9-horse results 1.jpg

Figure 3: Horses chose the attentive individual more than expected by chance in the body, head, and eye conditions.

These results indicate that horses are indeed skilled at reading human attentional cues, and are capable of perceiving both subtle cues (eye gaze) as well as grosser cues (such as body and head orientation) to do so. Since they were equally accurate across all three conditions, it appears as if they are equally able to utilize each different type of social information. The experimenters also collected response time information, and it turned out that response times were longer for incorrect trials, compared to correct trials, for the head and body conditions. They interpret this data such that the horses were less sure of their decision on these occasions. For the eye condition, however, there was no difference in response time. Given the above interpretation for the head and body conditions, this would seem to indicate that the horses did not have any underlying uncertainty.

i-957dad86c937abd2fa7e41424143fdcc-horse results 2.jpg

Figure 4: Response time data for each condition.

I find this interpretation to be highly speculative, though the difference in response time between correct and incorrect trials is intriguing, and particularly the difference in incorrect response time between conditions. The incorrect response time for the head and body conditions, while statistically different from the correct response time, also has also a lot more variance (they don't indicate what the error bars represent (FAIL!), but I'm guessing they are standard error). I'm at a loss to think of alternative explanations, however. What is also particularly interesting is that four of the eight subjects who approached the inattentive person in the body condition moved around to the front of the person, potentially to enter the experimenter's field of vision (again, highly speculative interpretation). They also used additional gestures such as nudging the inattentive experimenters to seek a food reward, suggesting that they may have been trying to get the attention of the experimenter.

One final result was that females were significantly more accurate than males in choosing the attentive experimenter, but all the experimenters provide is more speculation:

...long-lasting bonds between female horses are commonplace and in this sense horse societies have been described as matriarchal (Goodwin 1999; Jensen 2002; Wells and Goldschmidt-Rothschild 1979). This result would suggest that females may possess enhanced social discriminative abilities as has been reported in other matriarchal societies (McComb et al. 2000, 2001). However, recent research has not shown
domestic female horses to be more skilled than males at recognizing close associates (Proops et al. 2009), neither has this sex difference been reported in previous studies of
human attention attribution conducted with other species.

All of these are interesting speculations, but lots more research is necessary to pick apart these hypotheses. It is also unfortunate that the full extent of the horses' experience with humans was unknown. It will also be important to compare domestic horses with their wild cousins. So far, the only species in which these comparisons have been conducted are with dogs/wolves, and foxes.

Although the factors which give rise to this ability are likely to vary across taxa, the high numbers of correct responses on first trials, the lack of correlation between age and performance, and the accurate performance of some very young subjects in this study suggest that horses, like dogs, may have a predisposition to be highly sensitive to human attentional cues.

Despite its few shortcomings, this study, which is among the first conducted for horses, still provides a wealth of information, as well as a handful of new hypotheses. I am continually amazed by the wealth of information - and new questions - that can be generated based on simple behavioral data from within a cleverly designed experiment.

Now, sing it with me:

Proops, L., & McComb, K. (2009). Attributing attention: the use of human-given cues by domestic horses (Equus caballus) Animal Cognition, 13 (2), 197-205. DOI: 10.1007/s10071-009-0257-5

Categories

More like this

Dogs are particularly good at tasks that involve communicating or cooperating with humans, which has led some researchers to speculate that they are really good at solving social tasks, more generally. For example, dogs can figure out where a human's attention is, are really good at picking up on…
Cooperation and conflict are both a part of human society. While a good deal of the academic literature addresses the evolutionary origins of conflict, in recent years there has been an increased focus on the investigation of the evolutionary origins of cooperative behavior. One component of…
I will be reposting some dog-related posts from the archives in the coming few weeks as I prepare for the course I'm teaching this semester on dog cognition. Please let me know if you find something inaccurate or unclear. Domesticated dogs seem to have an uncanny ability to understand human…
Dogs are pretty smart. They can have huge vocabularies, they can infer meaning in the growls of other dogs, and they can effortlessly figure out if other dogs want to play or fight with them. But their intelligence might be limited to the social domain; indeed, while they outperform chimpanzees in…

Did they consider that the increased (neg) response time for turned away and looking away may reflect curiosity about *why* the experimenter turned away? In my personal experience of horses, they're very aware of one person in a group seeming interested in something no-one else has noticed yet. In particular, facing one way and looking the other would imply that you've just noticed something that is a potential threat.

By stripey_cat (not verified) on 26 Jul 2010 #permalink

This may be possible, though more research would be needed to address this. Several other species (including dogs) seem to be able to distinguish between humans looking away and just generally out into space versus looking away and actively attending to something salient in the environment.

Hmmm.. did these researchers ever think of asking a cowboy about horse behavior? You never approach a horse looking into it's eyes like predator! You move slowly toward it's shoulder, and touch it there lightly before going near the head.

They did not approach the horses, the horses approached them.

Bo- I'll take this from my own personal experience with horses, I worked with racing Standardbreds for awhile, and drove carriage horses in a major city for over 5 years.

Eye contact certainly tends to get a horse's attention, and when approaching an unfamiliar or nervous horse it certainly helps to look at its shoulder or back rather than head, but horses (as mentioned about) respond readily to body cues. If a person stands making eye contact with shoulders hunched, legs tense and head low, the horse will likely see that as a predatory stance. From the somewhat grainy pictures offered the experimenters were standing attentive but head up and relaxed, which many horses will take as an invitation to come and say hello.

My favorite way to catch a recalcitrant horse in the pasture was always to make eye contact and walk towards them, once they moved away a bit I would turn around and walk away. Usually the horse would catch up to me before I could get to the gate! This worked on many an experienced dodger, though sometimes you'd have to do it a couple of times to get them close enough. It rarely worked when I didn't make eye contact though, they'd move away but not follow after.

Jason- Thank you for this article! Hopefully this experiment will be repeated a few times, and with foals. I'd also like to see it repeated in a closed arena environment with fewer potential distractions to see if that changes the failure rate in way.

By ThisIsGrand (not verified) on 27 Jul 2010 #permalink

Interesting. I have ~50 years experience breeding, raising and breaking horses of several breeds and mixed ancestry, so I would consider myself an expert on aspects of horse behavior. Horses that are used to being around humans use the same physical cues that they do in their own social contexts to gather information on intention, moods, etc. In the wild they are herd animals with harems of females guarded by a single mature male. The females have a definite, permanent pecking order, that may be resented by the underlings, (fairness, justice and revenge are not just primate concepts!) but is obeyed. It is to the female's advantage to remain aware of the moods, etc. of the superior females in order to avoid injury. All the females except for the most aggressive, agile kickers, have to watch out for the resident stallion. As a stallion ages, it tends to become "rank", that is aggressive and even sadistic, often beating up on a mare for no particular reason, so an awareness of the moods and physical location of others is paramount.
That being said, they will learn in a VERY short time who has the tidbit and will harass you if you don't fork over.