Literary Darwinism?

Jonathon Gottschall, in a recent piece in New Scientist (reprinted here) offers what he calls "Literary Darwinism":

Understanding a story is ultimately about understanding the human mind. The primary job of the literary critic is to pry open the craniums of characters, authors and narrators, climb inside their heads and spelunk through the bewildering complexity within to figure out what makes them tick.

Yet, in doing this, literary scholars have ignored the recent scientific revolution that has transformed our understanding of why people behave the way they do. While evolutionary biologists have irreparably shattered the blank slate, most students of the humanities still insist that humans are born all but free of any innate qualities.

My fellow literary Darwinists and I hope to change their minds. By applying evolution-based thinking to fiction, we believe we can invigorate the study of literature, while at the same time mining an untapped source of information for the scientific study of human nature ... Darwinian thinking can help us better understand why characters act and think as they do, why plots and themes resonate within such very narrow bounds of variation, and the ultimate reasons for the human animal's strange, ardent love affair with stories.

So, what is this "Darwinian thinking" that Gottschall offers as the magic test of fiction? Is it natural selection? Is it phylogenetic diversification? Sexual selection? None of these, nor anything Darwin would have accepted. Instead, it is this (under the fold):

While the specifics of the 1960s bestseller [The Naked Ape by Desmond Morris] were outdated, its general attitude towards human behaviour was not. Morris argued that although humans have complicated culture and a stunning capacity to learn, this does not change the fact that we are also animals, vertebrates, mammals, primates and, ultimately, great apes.

Aspects of our culture and intelligence mean we are different from other apes but do not emancipate us from biology or lift us above other animals onto an exalted link of the chain of being. What's more, it follows that the behavioural characteristics of the human animal, just like the physical ones, should be understood as the products of a long evolutionary process. Morris did not claim this rendered all other perspectives on human behaviour obsolete, just that an important fact had been neglected to the detriment of our understanding: people are apes.

One should call it Literary Linneanism then, for it was that worthy who noticed this (and was attacked by Lutheran Bishops and others for so doing). What Darwin added to this was merely the expectation that as apes we should share many traits with our cousins, and his book The Expression of the Emotions is just such a discussion. But the distinction Gottschall relies on, that nature and nurture are opposed, was not Darwin's view. In fact, Darwin had no clear distinction between nature (innate traits) and nurture (acquired traits), which had to wait for the triumph of August Weismann's doctrine of the sequestration of the germline in heredity.

Gottschall notes that warriors in classical literature spend a lot of time competing for women, and here he is on firmer ground. But while this is true, is it especially Darwinian? In point of fact, the explication he gives of this is classical Marxian analysis - low status males sought to increase their access to resources including mates by battle. It is true, I think, that an evolutionary account can be given an economic flavour, and vice versa. And it is also true that we are apes, and they are very like us, as Frans de Waal's work indicates. At best, though, this is comparative ethology, not "Darwinism", which is, of course, what Morris was doing. Morris had been a student of Niko Tinbergen's.

We actually are in a better state now than in the 1960s and 1970s when Morris was popular, to make a fist of a biosocial account of human behaviour. I especially recommend the work of James Sidianus and Felicia Pratto on Social Dominance, which, despite being attacked as an explanation for immediate social behaviours (and I'm not sure it doesn't work there) is I think an excellent beginning into the explication of the social dominance hierarchy behaviours of that naked social ape. Clibing the dominance hierarchy is not merely chasing mates, or conspicuous consumption. We are guided in our behaviours by larger concerns than sex and money, and they all return to the question of what worked for us when we first evolved as we are now. In short, what was the fitness of these things once upon a time.

A species typically evolves in a given locale, and its adaptations are reinforced by compatibility with other traits or genetic dispositions. Hence, if it is successful at migrating into novel environments, these traits will tend to be retained, or modified only slightly. A lot of behaviours can therefore be inadaptive or even maladaptive. However, something worth noting about us human apes is that we do a lot of niche construction. The environment is adapted to us, not we to it. So if you were to consider fiction through the eyes of a modern ethologist, apart from needing to get the "innate" psychology right (i.e., what humans actually do), you also need to consider why they built the social context, the landscape, and the urban systems they do, just as Ferdinand Braudel noted without any aid from biology. [I might also mention the greatly underappreciated book by Charles Dyke listed at the end of this post in this connection.]

So what worth to history, fiction and sociology can biology have? I think there is a sense in which evolution can affect the ways we think about ourselves, and it is rather radical. In effect, evolution takes the pre-eminence of agency out of the picture - people can have all the intentions they wish, and all the rationales justifying their actions they can invent. It really doesn't matter for explaining how history goes. A smart intention may work, but so may a dumb one; an action may have intended effects, or it may not. The explanation that really matters, though, is why we do these things, intend these things, and justify these things the ways we do. And that boils down to the fact that we are social, hierarchical, resource gathering, sexually reproducing, apes.

So I will suggest that we call this "Literary Primatology", not "Darwinism", which is, as I have noted before, a word that means everything and nothing.

Dyke, C. 1988. The evolutionary dynamics of complex systems: a study in biosocial complexity, Monographs on the history and philosophy of biology. New York: Oxford University Press.

More like this

So now, I think it's worth asking what we really can achieve by doing sociobiological investigations, and some of the traps in previous attempts. Humans are animals. They are vertebrates, mammals, primates, and apes. Like other animals, their behaviours are formed, constrained, and in most cases…
This is the fourth of a series: Part 1, Part 2, Part 3 Wilson and Wilson (W&W) then continue on to employ some recent work on individuals as groups, and the "major transitions" literature. Ed Wilson is well known for his idea of "superorganisms", in which eusocial hives or colonies of…
A conference is being held in Sydney soon about whether God is necessary for morality. I find that an almost incomprehensible question. Of course humans are moral without gods to back up their moral systems. They can't help it. It's what humans do. We are social apes that follow rules. Sometimes…
Suppose you come from Mars, freshly minted with your PhD degree in the ethology of terrestrial mammals, and you decide to study this ape species that uses language and technology. Suppose further it's about 10,000 years before now. What would you describe? That is the topic of this post. It's…

"One should call it Literary Linneanism then, for it was that worthy who noticed this (and was attacked by Lutheran Bishops and others for so doing)."

Then one could still call it Darwinism because it was Granpappy Erasmus who did most to introduce the ideas of Linnaeus into England and thus the English language ;)

No, I'm afraid you are wrong. Botanists like James Edward Smith introduced Linnaean taxonomy and conventions, and it was taken up enthusiastically by gardeners.

Its Fernand Braudel, by the way.

I realise that several people were involved in the introduction of Linnean concepts into Britain but in fact Darwin and other members of the Lunar Society were promoting Linneaeus before the involvment of Smith who in fact studied under Joseph Black in Edinburgh an associate of Erasmus. In fact as Smith moved to London in 1783 to continue his studies Erasmus was translating Linnaeus' Systema Vegetabilium.

Well thanks to Thony and Benny for correcting my mistakes here. This is why I have a blog, so people can teach me.

I hadn't read Braudel for 25 years. An when I studied Erasmus Darwin, I failed to notice any link with Linnaeus.

I'm quite happy to admit that I know zilch about the history of biology and almost as little about the 18th century. (This statement is of course qualified by what I mean when I say, "know"!). My area is the mathematical sciences and that mostly in the 15th and 16th centuries. However I do try to avoid becoming what the Germans call a "Fachidiot" that is someone who is an expert in a narrow specialty but is otherwise ignorant. That is actually the main reason that I visit your web site everyday in order to learn something about the history and philosophy of biology.

One of my main 'outside' interests is the history of scientific societies and there my favourite society is the Lunar Society. I mean anyone, who doesn't find a purely amateur society for science and technology, whose members included both of Charles Darwin's grandfathers, Joseph Priestly, James Watt and Benjamin Franklin and whose nick name was 'The Lunatics', totally fascinating just aint interested in the history of science. Therefore I have read and very much enjoyed Jenny Uglow's The Lunar Men a book that I would heartily recommend to anybody. It contains quite a lot on the Lunar Society's interest in Linnaeus and in particular on Erasmus Darwin's involvement in spreading the Linnaean gospel. Whilst I am recommending books another popular book on the history of biology that I found really excellent is Matthew Cobb's "The Egg & Sperm Race: The Seventeenth-Century Scientists Who Unravelled the Secrets of Sex, Life and Growth", well worth a read.

Morris has a new book out (okay, I just saw a paperback edition yesterday, so not utterly new) called The Naked Woman: A Study of the Female Body. I passed it by, as the index indicated it omits what I think are the key questions of human female evolution ("concealed estrus", too-narrow birth canal, etc) - but I suspect an aspiring novelist might be able to find a useful tidbit or two therein, with much less risk, trouble & confusion than researching live specimens...

By Pierce R. Butler (not verified) on 25 Apr 2007 #permalink

Gottschall and David Sloan Wilson have co-edited a volume of essays on literary Darwinism that has a forward by E. O. Wilson: The Literary Animal: Evolution and the Nature of Narrative. I've published a skeptical review-essay where I juxtapose it against a very different book, Franco Moretti's Graphs, Maps, Trees: Abstract Models for a Literary History. I find this literary Darwinism to be rather thin, naive, and confused. It is also remarkably uninterested in analyzing and understanding the diversity of literary forms. Its Darwinism seems aimed at fixing human psychology in place (in its environment of evolutionary adaptedness) and glossing over variation and change in literature itself.

To the extent that it gives visibility to biologically-grounded psychology within literary studies, literary Darwinism is usefull. Otherwise it appears to be a dead end. When Gottschall reports on the Victorian novels study (I was one of the 500 people who took part in the survey, but I haven't seen the results) he indicates that the study showed:

First, that readers' responses reflect an evolved psychological tendency to envision human social relations as morally polarised struggles between "us" and "them". Protagonists and their allies form co-operative communities that readers empathise with and participate vicariously in. By contrast, readers tend to view antagonists and their allies as an "out-group" - a malign force, motivated by a desire for social dominance as an end in itself, that threatens the very principle of community.

There's nothing new here except some of the terminology. In this respect literary Darwinism is a bit like "memetics," restating existing ideas in new terminology without, however, yielding new insight.

I could go on and on, but there's no point in doing that in this commment as I've already done that in my review-essay.

By Bill Benzon (not verified) on 26 Apr 2007 #permalink