Fearful facial expressions enhance our perception

Blogging on Peer-Reviewed ResearchShow someone a piece of rotting food and their reaction will be visibly similar the world over. Their eyes will close, their noses will wrinkle and their mouths will tighten, all part of a universal expression of disgust. Darwin himself was struck by the universal nature of human facial expressions - from the busiest of cities to the most isolated of villages, smiles and frowns are recognisable, done in the same way and carry the same meaning.

i-fd500dcb630df62e8117c59c5a44faed-Scream.jpgFacial expressions are massively important for the social lives of humans and it should come as no surprise that some parts of our brain are dedicated to interpreting the small shifts in facial muscles that betray our emotions. In this light, it makes sense that expressions should be universal, but that doesn't tell us why they take the form they do. Why is it a smile that indicates happiness and not anger, or why should wide eyes signify fear or surprise and not disgust?

A new study reveals part of the answer and shows that it's not an arbitrary fluke that certain emotions are tied to specific movements of our facial muscles. Joshua Susskind from the University of Toronto has found that expressions of fear and disgust are adaptive and serve to alter the experiences of our senses.

Fear creates a need for watchfulness to detect potential threats and fearful expressions help with this by widening the eyes and nostrils to increase the information available to the senses. The opposite is true for disgust, which is more about rejecting information that you don't want. And accordingly, disgusted expressions serve to restrict our perceptions.

Suskind first demonstrated the polar nature of fear and disgust by taking images of Japanese and Caucasian facial expressions from a large library. He plugged them into a well-known computer model that averaged out their features to create standard faces that epitomised different emotions. The model also worked out how the shape of these standard faces, and the way they reflect light, changes along their surface. It used this information to create the polar opposites of certain expressions - a set of 'anti-faces'.

i-469900527d67482f6a54d7781db551fc-Faceantiface.jpg

Remarkably, the opposite facial expression to fear turned out to be very much like disgust, and vice versa, even to the eyes of impartial observers. A fearful expression is effectively the opposite of a disgusted one. The former is very much about expanding features like the eyes, nostrils and mouth, while the latter involves scrunching these up.

The eyes have it

i-c79b9330e0cfe61acc75da01eb5adef0-Vision.jpgSuskind asked 20 students to make fearful or disgusted faces while looking at a large grid. He found that the students who pretended to be scared had a larger field of vision than normal, while the mock-disgusted students had a smaller view of the world. As a further test, Suskind asked volunteers to fixate on the centre of a projection screen, while a gray dot flashed ever closer to the centre of their field of vision. Again, expressions of fear gave the students the ability to spot the dot at a greater distance than expressions of disgust did.

Fear enlarges a person's field of vision but it also allows them to scan it more effectively using faster flicks of the eye called saccades. When volunteers had to rapidly switch their gaze between two small circles, the fearful ones moved their eyes much more quickly than their disgusted peers.

i-4ac5bbafac72a42af5b1e0049cfc9d7b-Nasal.jpgSight is not the only sense that's affected - smell gets a boost from fear too. Using airflow meters, Suskind showed that people who look afraid can inhale larger volumes of air than normal, and MRI scans showed that this is due to a widening of their nasal passages. The same passages are sealed off by disgusted expressions, and inhalation volume falls as a result.

The results cannot prove that facial expressions once gave their wearers any adaptive benefits. But they do at least provide support for the idea that our expressions originally developed to modify our perceptions, an idea first proposed by Darwin over a century ago. You can see similar things in other primates, many of which reflexively close their eyes and flatten their eyes when they are startled.

Suskind suggests that these adaptive expressions were then recruited for the purposes of social communication. They have developed as social signals over the course of human evolution and have probably changed substantially during that time, for a variety of reasons. But as these experiments show, they still retain a vestige of their original purposes.

Reference: Susskind, J.M., Lee, D.H., Cusi, A., Feiman, R., Grabski, W., Anderson, A.K. (2008). Expressing fear enhances sensory acquisition. Nature Neuroscience DOI: 10.1038/nn.2138

Images courtesy of Nature.

More like this

Human beings use stereotyped facial expressions to identify the feelings of others. We can tell what another person is feeling in part because of how their face looks. However, this says very little about why the particular changes in facial musculature are associated with particular feelings.…
Both objects and behaviour can be described as disgusting. The term could equally apply to someone who cheats other people out of money as it could to the sight of rancid food or the taste of sour milk. That's not just a linguistic quirk. Some scientists believe that the revulsion we feel towards…
Take a look at these two faces. One of these women can't recognize that the other is afraid, but when asked to express fear, is still able to produce a fearful expression. Can you tell which is which? We know the amygdala is associated with identifying scary music; we know the amygdala helps us…
Darwin's spirit lives on in everything from the Human Genome Project to medicine to conservation biology--the three topics I covered in my post on Friday. It also lives on in brain scans. While Darwin is best known for The Origin of Species, he also wrote a lot of books in later years, most of…

As it stands, this research is rather panglossian - it looks for and finds practical reasons for certain emotions producing certain expressions.

BUT...are there for instance some possible expressions of fear that would serve some fear-related purpose better? If so, why have these better expressions been evolved?

Are there some expressions that do the opposite - pulling the face into contortions that are counterproductive with regards to their stimulus? I'd have thought screaming in terror could be analysed as counterproductive, because it leaves the mouth and throat open to attack.

Are there cases where, for instance, a wrinkling of the nose and a widening of the eyes would both be useful, but because of the structure of the face, only one can be achieved?

And are there expressions which cannot reasonably be said to have a physiological function at all. An incredulous stare widens the mouth and eyes, but why would an open mouth be "helpful" when faced with something inexplicable?

I suggest the primary evolutionary advantage of screaming in terror is that it alerts other members of our species to danger. I read, years ago, that as people lose hearing with age, there is one pitch that remains clearly audible the longest... which happens to be the same pitch as the typical human scream of terror. Alerting the tribe to danger is valuable.
But think the opposite of fear looked like a laughing man, and the opposite of disgust looked like sorrowing remorse.

By Samantha Vimes (not verified) on 23 Jun 2008 #permalink