Gestures and learning: Using gestures -- not just seeing them -- can help kids learn

ResearchBlogging.orgI was a member of my high school debate team, and I did fairly well, but my partner, Glenn, always got better marks from the judges. Most often, they praised his hand gestures, which were proclaimed to be "expressive" and "informative." One year our topic was arms control, and our opponents were arguing that "NATO standardization" could help reduce U.S. arms sales. Glenn didn't understand their argument, so during our precious three minutes of preparation time, I explained it to him. Then he stood up and delivered his rebuttal, using the most graceful hand gestures imaginable. The judges unanimously said on their ballots that they thought Glenn understood the arguments better than any of the debaters on either side.

Could those hand gestures really be the key to Glenn's high marks from the judges? There's plenty of research suggesting that informative hand gestures make speech easier to understand. But additional research also suggests that the person doing the gesturing can learn more effectively than someone who doesn't gesture while learning. Maybe during the course of his speech, Glenn actually did come to understand the argument better than anyone in the room.

A team led by Susan Wagner Cook took 84 third- and fourth-graders and tested them to see if they could do a simple type of math problem, like this:

4 + 3 + 6 = ___ + 6

Selecting only the children who didn't yet understand how to solve this sort of problem, they worked one-on-one with the kids, modeling one of three methods to solve it: either using gestures, using speech and gestures, or using speech alone. Then everyone was taught the same method of solving using both speech and gestures:

For example, after putting the correct answer in the blank for the problem 4 + 9 + 3 = 4 + ___, the instructor said, "I want to make one side (while sweeping the left hand under the left side of the equation) equal to the other side (while sweeping the right hand under the right side); so four plus nine plus three equals sixteen, and four plus twelve equals sixteen; one side (gestures under the left side) is equal to the other side (gestures under the right side).

This was explained for six different problems, after each of which the child was asked to solve a problem of his or her own, mimicking the behavior they had seen at the outset (gestures alone, speech and gestures, or speech alone). The children were then given a post-test on a new set of problems. Finally, everyone was given a follow-up four weeks later. Here are the results:

i-1be1ab23d7615beba809e0856ca0d189-cook1.gif

These graphs can be a little confusing at first, but they are very revealing. On the post-test given immediately after they learned to solve the problem, everyone's scores were roughly the same: Some students solved all the problems, and some solved none, with only a few in the middle of the scale. But on the follow-up four weeks later, big differences emerged. Kids who had solved their practice problems using either gesture alone or gesture and speech solved about the same number of problems in the follow-up as they had in the post-test. But many of the kids who had solved problems correctly in the speech-only group didn't fare as well on the follow-up; only a few scored well on both the post-test and the follow-up.

Cook's team says this difference must be due to the students' own gesturing. Since all the students had been taught using both speech and gestures, their own gestures must have been responsible for the learning.

How does gesture do this? At this point, we can only speculate. Perhaps gestures are relatively easy, freeing cognitive resources for other tasks. Maybe gestures help us encode thoughts into memory. Or maybe they help build links to the external environment, which in turn makes things easier to synthesize and recall.

COOK, S., MITCHELL, Z., & GOLDINMEADOW, S. (2008). Gesturing makes learning last Cognition, 106 (2), 1047-1058 DOI: 10.1016/j.cognition.2007.04.010

More like this

A couple of "kids these days are bad at math" stories crossed my feed reader last week, first a New York Times blog post about remedial math, then a Cocktail Party Physics post on confusion about equals signs. The first was brought to my attention via a locked LiveJournal post taking the obligatory…
One of the more contentious recurring topics around here over the years has been education policy, mostly centering around the question of teacher evaluation and teacher's unions. It's probably the subject for which there's the biggest gap between my opinions and those of some of my regular readers…
As noted in previous posts, I've been trying something radically different with this term's classes, working to minimize the time I spend lecturing, and replace it with in-class discussion and "clicker questions." I'm typing this while proctoring the final exam for the second of the two classes I'm…
Here's a particularly worthless article from the AP: Docs say Tamiflu won't affect foetus. This is clearly an important question. In the event of a pandemic, Tamflu will be used prophylactically in pregnant women, either by choice or because the women don't know they are pregnant. There is…

It's possible that gesturing while learning adds in the "doing" aspect of comprehension so their links to what they've learned are more solid.

I think an important factor in learning is attention and focus. Perhaps by using gestures, the child is more engaged with the problem, making the difference.

I have twins children. When time came for them to learn reading one was in a class where the teacher was using the standard read/write/spell approach while the other's teacher was using a method whereby all sounds and writing were associated with hand movement (a method I was told initially developed for dyslexic people).

The result was quite astonishing at the end of the year. The one using gestures was far better at reading than the one who has been taught the standard way. I observed also that on average classmate of my "gesture" kid were better at reading than classmate of my "classic" kid.

I believe, that the explanation can be quite easy.
Children learns with their whole body (it's unity of body and mind that strongly express during childhood and comes back for old age) so they can better remember while learning mixture of words and movements. It is connected to the fact that when a child is moving their brains connections develops more efficiently (especially commissura magna). New 'ways' in brain are created, moreover they are stronger (as they are consolidated both with motion and speech).
The second explantion can be made basing on NLP model - some people are considered 'Kinesthetic', so learn much easier while moving (it's a kind of 'anchor', http://en.wikipedia.org/wiki/Neuro-linguistic_programming#Anchoring). Due to it children can have tendency to use their kinestetic abilities, but during the 'normal' shool proces it is just lost.
Still there can be some other explanations that are yet to be discovered. I look forward to it as it's really interesting for me.

It also could be that the groups using gestures only strengthened the learning of those whom could have learned equally as well without gestures, but also added additional learners whom needed the visual relational element. To me this implies rather than kinesthetic learning, the children were aided by a visual learning element. The learning seems to have made a more concrete connection to the children via a visual element of "seeing" the math done. I think it was not the actual physical gestures, learning with a whole body approach, via kinesthetic learning, but visual.

By jlorraine (not verified) on 24 Apr 2009 #permalink