Babies with Grown-up Brains

The soft spot on a baby's head may be able to tell us when our ancestors first began to speak.

We have tremendously huge brains--six times bigger than the typical brain of a mammal our size. Obviously, that big size brings some fabulous benefits--consciousness, reasoning, and so on. But it has forced a drastic reorganization of the way we grow up. Most primates are born with a brain fairly close to its adult size. A macaque brain, for example, is 70% of adult size at birth. Apes, on the other hand, have bigger brains, and more of their brain growth takes place after birth. A chimpanzee is born with a brain 40% of its adult size, and by the end of its first year it has reached 80% of adult size. Humans have taken this trend to an almost absurd extreme. We are born with brains that are only 25% the size of an adult brain. By the end of our first year, our brains have reached only 50%. Even at age 10, our brains are not done growing, having reached 95% of adult size. For over a decade, in other words, we have newborn brains.

It's likely that this growth pattern evolved as a solution to a paradox of pregnancy. Brains demand huge amounts of energy. If mothers were to give birth to babies with adult-sized brains, they would have to supply their unborn children with a lot more calories in utero. Moreover, childbirth is already a tight fit that can put a mother's life in jeopardy. Expand the baby's head more, and you raise the risks even higher.

Extending the growth of the brain obviously gave us big brains, but it may have endowed us with another gift. All that growth now happened not in the dark confines of the womb, but over the course of years of childhood. Instead of floating in an aminotic sac, children run around, fall off chairs, bang on pots, and see how loud they can scream. (At least mine do.) In other words, they are experiencing what it's like to control their body in the outside world. And because their brains are still developing, they can easily make new connections to learn from these experiences. Some researchers even argue that only after the brains of our ancestors became plastic was it possible for them to begin to use language. After all, language is one of the most important things that children learn, and they do a far better job of learning it than adults do. If scientists could somehow find a marker in hominid fossils that shows how their brains grew, it might be possible to put a date on the origin of language.

That's where the soft spot comes in.

The oldest hominids that look anything like humans first emerged in Africa about 2 million years ago. They were about as tall as us, with long legs and arms, narrow rib cages, flat faces, and small teeth. The earliest of these human-like hominids are known as Homo ergaster, but they rapidly gave rise to a long-lived species called Homo erectus. H. erectus probably originated in Africa, but then burst out of the home continent and spread across Asia to Indonesia and China. The Homo erectus people who stayed behind in Africa are probably our own ancestors. The Asian H. erectus thrived until less than 100,000 years ago. They could make simple stone axes and choppers, and had brains about two-thirds the size of ours.

Paleoanthropologists have found only a single braincase of a baby Homo erectus. It was discovered in Indonesia in 1936, and has since been dated to 1.8 million years old--close to the origin of the species. While scientists have had a long time to study it, they haven't made a lot of progress. One problem is that the fossil lacks jaws or teeth, which can offer clues to the age of a hominid skull. The other problem is that the interior of the braincase was filled with rock, making it hard to chart its anatomy.

In the new issue of Nature, a team of researchers rectified this problem with the help of a CT scanner. They were able to calculate the volume of the child's brain, and then they were able to map the bones of the skull more accurately. As babies grow, the soft spot on their skull closes up and other bones are also rearranged in a predictable sequence. Chimpanzees, our closest living relatives, also close up their skulls in the same pattern, with some small differences in timing. The H. erectus baby, its skull shows, was somewhere between six and eighteen months old. Despite its tender age, the Homo erectus baby had a big brain--84% the size of adult Homo erectus brains as measured in fossil skulls.

A single battered braincase still leaves plenty of room for uncertainty, but it's still a pretty astonishing result. At a year old, this Homo erectus baby was almost finished growing its brain. It spent very little time developing its brain outside the womb, suggesting that it didn't have enough opportunity to develop the sophisticated sort of thinking that modern human children do. If that's true, then it's unlikely it could ever learn to speak. If these researchers are right, then future CT scans of younger hominid skulls should be able to track the rise of our long childhood.

Tags

More like this

At 1 p.m. today I listened by phone to a press conference in Washington where scientists presented the first good look inside a Hobbit's head. The view is fascinating. While it may help clear up some mysteries, it seems to throw others wide open. Last October, a team of Australian scientists…
Get to know that little skull. Scientists are going to be talking about it for centuries. As researchers report in tomorrow's issue of Nature, the skull--and along with other parts of a skeleton--turned up in a cave on the Indonesian island of Flores. Several different dating methods gave the same…
Two years ago this month, I was taken aback by some explosive news. A team of Indonesian and Australian scientists reported that they had discovered fossils of what they claimed was a new species of hominid. It lived on the island of Flores in Indonesia, it stood three feet tall, and it had a brain…
In 1970, the natural history illustrator Rudolph Zallinger painted a picture of human evolution called "The March of Progress" in which a parade of hominids walked along from left to right, evolving from knuckle-walking ape to tall, spear-carrying Cro-Magnon. The picture is etched in our collective…

Reminds me of something I think Robert Wright mentioned in The Moral Animal; that humans develop slowly due to an inefficient birthing process with its roots in walking upright, which gave women narrower hips so babies had to be born smaller.

If so, the first humans to be "erectus" might have still given birth at full brain size, giving even more importance to the development of walking upright, which now seems to have given us childhood. Very cool.

Interesting that Homo erectus might have a similar brain growth/age profile as that of the chimpanzee. Makes one wonder if such profiles if they could ever be created from sufficient data points from living and fossil evidence might be a co-indicator of ancestry among homonids and other primates.

Makes me wonder if the onset of puberty and brain growth also correlate and how that works into the social structure of various primates. Probably a study out there somewhere; and it might actually be seen as more a coincidence.

And why, asks the innocent child, is my journey so long? This 'Time' we see as development is but the continuous unlayering of the onion we fuzzingly refer to as Truth. It's an Undoing of what we thought we knew, a willingness to say "at least I can decide I do not like the way I feel." And thus the journey advances one more step closer to its Source.
Celine Dione sings, 'It's never to long to find the way home," acknowleding our self made illusion of Time. This Instant is the only Time there is. We dream, but we must bless the dream to awaken.

Bill

By William Gruzenski (not verified) on 17 Sep 2004 #permalink

The assumption made by this article is that language is more easily learned by babies because they have some increased capacity to learn language and not because of daily hours of exposure.

If an adult is completely emersed in a language they will learn the language just as fast as an infant, perhaps even a little faster. True, their pronunciation and deeper understandings may take a little extra work, like speech therapy and extra training. But your typical child is corrected and taught by its parents, where this type of correction seldom takes place with adults.

An adult will be as good at a language they learn later in life as they choose to be, why couldn't Homo Erectus children, with more developed brains, have made this same choice?

By Jeff Dodds (not verified) on 07 Oct 2004 #permalink

Jeff you missed the point. Not native language but any language, is easier learned by children.
Bill, what made you brain stop growing and think Celine was a philosopher? Bottom line is, evolution is.