Cool Visual Illusions: Mach Bands

Discovered in the 1860s by Ernst Mach (hence the name), Mach Bands are actually a set of interrelated phenomena. Take a look at this image:

i-0a8bd597f3e286ea2134f65dbcc08ab0-machbands.bmp

From here

The individual bands should appear as gradients, and they may even appear to be curved. In fact, they are all solid colors. Now look at this one:

i-0719dfe990c69b4c7e4a1d4ac7fd2496-purvesmach3.bmp

From here

If you look closely at the area above the center two arrows, you should see a thin bright line (left-middle arrow) and a thin dark line (right-middle arrow). Once again, this is despite the fact that each of the three areas (dark, light, and in between) are solid colors.

This figure (from here) will aid in explaining these phenomena:

i-e23995f934c9b3d4742d580b63d5a489-machbandscentersurround.bmp

In the graph on the bottom, the black line represents the actual luminance of the figure. The red line reflects the perceived luminance. The red line's deviation from the black line represents the Mach Band phenomena, and the little spikes over and above the black line represent the bright and dark lines you see in the second figure above.

Why does this occur? The traditional, and still generally accepted explanation goes something like this. Light reflecting from each of the colored bands in the figures above will strike many different center-surround receptive fields (represented by the circles with + and - signs in the figure above the graph). Some of these receptive fields will actually overlap the boundaries of the bands. This causes these fields to be excited at different rates than if they received light only from one of the bands. These differing firing rates result in the fields sending signals that cause the visual system to believe slightly different luminances are present, resulting in the gradient, and at the edges, the bright or dark lines.

However, this explanation has been challenged recently by Dale Purves and his colleagues1. They argue that Mach Bands are actually the result of the properties of reflected light and the frequency with which we encounter curved surfaces in the environment. Because of the properties of reflected light, curved sufraces typically exhibit what Purves and his colleagues call "highlights" and "lowlights," which are essentially the same as Mach Bands. They argue that Mach Band phenomena therefore result from the visual system being tricked into thinking it's looking at a curved surface. To demonstrate this, they have shown that Mach Bands are stronger when the visual context is such that the illusion of a curved surface is stronger, and they are weaker when the context makes it clear that the image is flat. They argue that the traditional receptive-field explanation can't account for these results, and that instead the results indicate that the visual system has adapted to perceive "highlights" and "lowlights" on curved surfaces, and Mach Band-producing stimuli like those above activate this property of our visual system.


1Lotto, R.B., Williams, S.M., & Purves, D. (1999). Mach bands as empirically derived associations. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences, 96, 5245-5250.

More like this

This week's visual illusion is related to Mach bands, and similar in some ways to the watercolor effect. It's called the Craik-O'Brien-Cornsweet effect (or just the Cornsweet effect)1. This is the best example I've ever seen (from here): What you should see is a dark square over a light square (…
In honor of the announcement of the Best Visual Illusion of the Year (via Steve), I thought I'd revive the old cool visual illusion series. I may post about this year's winner, the leaning tower illusion, in the future, but I just now read the paper, so I have some work to do first. Instead, I'm…
Today for Monday Pets, we're going to go old school and talk about vision. Vision is arguably our most (intentionally) utilized sensory system, so its pretty important to figure out how it works. And it's what David Hubel and Torsten Wiesel set out to investigate starting in the late 1950s.…
Take a look at this amazing illusion created by Arthur Shapiro (you'll need the latest version of Flash Player to see it): You're looking at two donut-shaped figures whose "holes" are gradually changing color from black to white and back again. It appears that the holes are changing in an opposite…