The guardians of fear - molecules that provide safety nets for scary memories

As sufferers of post-traumatic stress syndrome know all too well, frightening experiences can be strong, long-lasting and notoriously difficult to erase. Now, we're starting to understand why. Far from trying to purge these memories, the brain actively protects them by hiring a group of molecular bodyguards called CSPGs (or chondroitin sulphate proteoglycans in full).

i-5ace3f51e5fb3a4205ddbd4cf921e1d2-Fear.jpgBy studying the brains of rats, Nadine Gogolla from Harvard University found that CSPGs - large chains of sugars and proteins - accumulate in the space around nerve cells and form defensive nets around a select few. Dissolve these nets, and the rats' fearful memories were more easily erased.

The nets start to form round about the time when rats reach adulthood and their fearful memories become harder to erase. As adults, rats can learn to be scared of an inoffensive sensation, like the sound of a buzzer, for the rest of their lives, if it's paired with an unpleasant one, like an electric shock.

However, the strength of this terror starts to wane if the rats repeatedly hear the ominous buzz without any nasty consequences. This process is called extinction, but it's nowhere near as permanent or robust as the creation of the original fear. The minute the shock returns, the fear response recovers. This happens so quickly that the memory clearly hadn't been erased or overwritten - the rat had merely learned to block it out.

Things are different in puphood. Before their third week of life, a rat's fear memories can be easily erased; only afterwards do they become indelible. Gogolla thinks that the nets are the reason why. She looked at the brains of baby rats in their first month of life, and focused on their amygdala, a pair of almond-shaped structures that have roles in processing emotions. The number of CSPG nets in these regions shot up as the days went past, but particularly in the third week, when the switch from erasable fear memories to permanent ones takes place. That's no coincidence.

Using electrified floor panels, Gogolla trained the mice to fear a burst of white noise at different ages. All of them quickly picked up this new dread, and they all of them lost it if the white noise was played on its own. But for those who were trained before the 16-day mark, before most of their CPSG nets had formed, the memory stayed gone even when the shocks were reintroduced. However, for mice who were trained after the 23-day mark, their dormant fears came easily rushing back. 

i-6e5c7cb5bcf5fd8e4050a6f23c5651d4-Rat-memories.jpg

To clearly demonstrate the protection provided by the nets, Gogolla set about destroying them. She injected 3-month-old rats with an enzyme called chondroitinase ABC that breaks down CSPGs. A day later, all the nets had gone, as had the permanent nature of the rats' fear memories. They could learn to fear white noise just as other adults did, but the destruction of their nets had reset them to their childhood state when their fears could be easily and rapidly deleted. All it took was a couple of bursts of painless white noise, and the rats' fears vanished completely.

Gogolla found that the net-degrading injections only worked if they were given to the animals before their initial bout of fearful training. This means that the protective nets (and by extension, the enzymes that destroy them) fundamentally change the way that we lay memories down in the brain. With the nets, the memories are encoded in a way that later makes them difficult to erase.

We store these memories (at least in part) by strengthening the connections (or synapses) between different nerve cells. This strengthening process is known as "long-term potentiation" or LTP, and it was severely weakened by the injections that knocked out the CSPG nets. This implies that the nets affect how strongly fear memories are stored in the first place; they could also physically protect the strengthened synapses once they are formed.

The fact that the anti-net injections don't work on existing memories is absolutely critical. It means that you can't use these enzymes to get rid of fearful memories that rats already have. They can only act as a preventative measure against building permanent versions of such memories in the future. Even if the same thing worked in a human brain, we wouldn't have a ready-made way of erasing traumatic memories. At most, it could provide a way for preventing permanent fears from consolidating in the minds of people who are prone to such things, such as those who suffer from anxiety disorders.

Reference: Science 10.1126/science.1174146 and 10.1126/science.1179697

Images: Scared child by D Sharon Pruitt; graph by AAAS/Science

More on fear memories:

More like this

A couple of weeks ago, I wrote about propranolol, a drug that can erase the emotion of fearful memories.  When volunteers take the drug before recalling a scary memory about a spider, it dulled the emotional sting of future recollections. It's not, however, a mind-wiping pill in the traditional…
The wiping of unwanted memories is a common staple of science-fiction and if you believe this weekend's headlines, you might think that the prospect has just become a reality. The Press Association said that a "drug helps erase fearful memories", while the ever-hyperbolic Daily Mail talked about a…
Bringing an old memory back to mind would, you might think, strengthen it. But not so - when memories are recalled, they enter a surprisingly vulnerable state, when they can be reshaped or even rewritten. It takes a while for the memory to become strengthened anew, through a process called…
Fear, that most primitive of emotions, is good, at least when it is kept under control. It is essential for survival, allowing an organism to detect a potential threat to its life. Too much fear, however, can lead to pathological conditions such as anxiety, phobia, paranoia, or post-traumatic…

If CSPGs only facilitate the formation of (fear) memories, then why does this article claim that they are protecting those memories? The last few paragraphs suggest that after the memory has formed (strongly), the CSPGs role has played out (for that memory). An analogy with bodyguards is made, but it seems more like these proteines act like martial arts trainers (or whatever), since they teach the memory to take care of itself (and therefor not be erased easily). Take away someones bodyguards and you can harm him, take away someones MA trainers and he will still kick your ass.

I also wonder if this result is unique to memories of fear and what this means with respect to the effect of age on learning. Children seem to learn faster and easier, but this research also seems to suggest that they will forget things easier than adults. Conversely, adults may learn slower but once they do, the memory lasts.
Television series have told me that people going to psychiatrists are often suffering from their problems because of childhood issues. This may be a stretch, but shouldn't this research suggest that such memories should have been forgotten, or at least that painful events encountered as an adult should have resulted in more potent fearful/unpleasant memories?

Jordi: according to this research, the CSPGs don't only facilitate the formation of memories; they create a protective net around the neurons associated with those memories. They don't "teach" the memories anything, but instead seem to physically protect the shape of the memory from subsequent extinction.

Which leads to your second point: This research isn't speaking to forgetfulness as a whole, or to the lasting impact of a memory once formed. It specifically approaches extinction, and how extinction is hampered by the existence of CSPG nets. The lack of CSPG nets in childhood means that extinction is significantly easier during the formative years. This doesn't directly mean that important memories formed in childhood will disappear, nor that memories formed later in life will have greater potency due to the CSPG nets.

Another thing we should take into account when considering your second point is that there are many more factors of memory encoding, storage, recollection and extinction than simply that which we see on the neurological level - the psychological aspects of memory are very important, as are the connections between memories and learned knowledge. Something traumatic that I experience as a child may be greater in intensity given my lack of experience in the world. If I experience something roughly similar in level of trauma in my later life, I may have enough experience with the nature of existence to realize the causes of that experience and thus temper it with wisdom, experience and cynicism such that it becomes a less traumatic memory.

This is my interpretation, and as such is potentially riddled with flaws, errors, omissions and delusions.

-C

Could this possibly have any connection to dissociative identity disorder, wherein a person walls off repeated traumatic behavior to create an isolated "alter" identity to protect the stability of the psyche as a whole?