The genetics of synaesthesia

When Sir Francis Galton first described the "peculiar habit of mind" we now call synaesthesia, he noted that it often runs in families. Modern techniques have confirmed that the condition does indeed have a strong genetic component - more than 40% of synaesthetes have a first-degree relative - a parent, sibling or offspring - who also has synaesthesia, and families often contain multiple synaesthetes.

Synaesthesia is known to affect females more than males, and although the female predominance of the condition is now known to have been exaggerated, the condition is presumed to be linked to the X chromosome. A number of genetic studies also support the theory that a single gene is responsible for synaesthesia, and that it is inherited in a dominant manner (in other words, just one copy of the gene, inherited from either parent, is sufficient to cause it).   

Researchers from the University of Oxford have now conducted the first genome-wide search for genes linked to the condition. In the American Journal of Human Genetics, they report the identification of a number of genes that are likely to be involved in auditory-visual synaesthesia, in which sounds are perceived as colours. The study reveals  also that synaesthesia is not X-linked, and that the genetics of this form of synaesthesia - and probably that of other forms - is far more complex than previously thought. 

A group led by Julian Asher of the Wellcome Trust Centre for Human Genetics, in collaboration with Simon Baron-Cohen's group at Cambridge, studied 43 large families, all of which include multiple members with auditory-visual synaesthesia. They recruited a total of 196 individuals, of which 121 were synaesthetes as confirmed by a questionnaire designed to test for the intensity and genuinesness of the synaesthetic experience. 

The researchers obtained DNA samples from each participant, and analysed more than 400 microsatellites dispersed across all the chromosomes. Microsatellites consist of very short sequences which are repeated multiple times; each allele, or variant, of a given gene contains a unique number of repeats, and this number often varies between individuals. These sequences are therefore often used to identify genetic variation in humans, as different alleles of the same gene can be distinguished from one another. In this case, however, the researchers searched for evidence of genetic linkage.

By comparing the DNA samples from different generations of synaesthetes from the same family, they identified the microsatellites which are inherited together. Rather than identifying specific genes, this analysis identified four distinct chromosomal regions located on three different chromosomes, all containing genes of interest. These regions are known to contain genes associated with a variety of disorders, including autism, dyslexia and epilepsy.

One of the candidate genes encodes the transcription factor TBR1, which regulates the activity of a number of other genes, including reelin, a signalling protein that is critical for the proper development of the cerebral cortex; another plays a role in several different developmental processes, including axon guidance, the process by which extending neuronal processes find their correct destination; and a third candidate, a sodium channel protein called SCN2A, is involved in regulating the electrical actvity of nerve cells and has been implicated in epilepsy. The region with the strongest linkage, which located on chromosome 2, is known to contain a gene associated with autism. Like synaesthesia, autism involves sensory and perceptual abnormalities, and autistics often report synaesthesia-like symptoms. 

Neuroimaging shows that the connections between the brain's sensory pathways are both denser and more active in synaesthetes than in non-synaesthetes. The condition is now viewed as being developmental in origin, and it is thought that newly-established connections, which would otherwise be "pruned" during development, remain in place, and perhaps become overactive. The results of the new study therefore fit nicely with current thinking about the neural bases of synaesthesia. Specific combinations of alleles of the identified candidate genes could feasibly lead to subtle changes in  developmental processes which ultimately result in alterations in neural architecture and activity thought to be involved in the condition.

As well as revealing the complexity of the genetics of synaesthesia, this study also shows that it can be inherited in a number of different ways (that is, by inheriting different combinations of alleles). The eventual identification and proper classification of all the candidate genes will inevitably lead to a better understanding not only of synaesthesia and the other conditions with which they are associated, but also of their roles in cognition more generally. The authors also suggest that advances in our knowledge of synaesthetic perceptions "may even shed light on the neural basis of consciousness".

Related: 


Asher, J. E. et al (2009). A Whole-Genome Scan and Fine-Mapping Linkage Study of Auditory-Visual Synesthesia Reveals Evidence of Linkage to Chromosomes 2q24, 5q33, 6p12, and 12p12.  Am. J. Hum. Genet. DOI: 10.1016/j.ajhg.2009.01.012.

More like this

I'm probably not qualified to make observations on this topic, but I persist in reading about it because I think I might be synaesthetic myself (I'm not sure how one would go about being definitively 'diagnosed').

Reading through your post, I get the distinct impression that we may all be synaesthetic at birth, but that the connections 'atrophy' over time as only those correlations that fit with our cultural mores are encouraged and reinforced in much the same way as we teach our children manners by reward reinforcement of acceptable behaviours and active discouragement of the unacceptable.

Before a child is able to verbalise their correlations between sounds and colours, the parents may inadvertently have begun the process of diminishing those connections just in the way they teach their children the vocabulary associated with shapes, numbers, sounds and colours. But let's say that the connections persist in sufficient strength until the child is able to move dreamily to a piece of music and say "Blue!" or clamp his hands over his ears and say "Thorny!" Unless synaesthesia is on the parents' radar, they are likely to 'correct' the child's pronouncement, because that's what good parents do. "No dear. This is music, it doesn't have colours." "Not thorny, dear. Loud."

When I was a child, I didn't eat any cabbage-related vegetables because they all tasted greyish. I imagine when I said, "I don't like this, it's grey," I caused offence to whoever had cooked the vegetables! As I grew up, the nuanced shades of flavours became more defined and I realised that Brussels sprouts actually taste olive green, which I now enjoy. White cabbage still tastes grey, but it is a light grey, with a hint of light green about it and not unpleasant at all. Green cabbage tastes a deeper green with a hint of softening grey about it.

However, most foods taste of colours for which I have no name because the texture and reflectiveness of the 'colour' are as much a component of its definition as the colour itself.

This article interested me because I come from a family where many of us are both dyslexic and have varying forms of synaesthesia, and problems with language development.
My daughter-in-law has none of these, so she recorded the colours we used for numbers up to ten and the days of the week for two generations. About three years later she repeated this and found that, not only had the colours not changed, but each person usually used the same phrases to describe the colours e.g. 3 is yellow, the colour of primroses whilst 6 is the yellow of daffodils. This result would make sense if the colours are hard-wired during development, and explain why siblings (including my twins) have different colours for the same numbers even though they used the same books etc as children?

By Revd. Dr Jessi… (not verified) on 06 Feb 2009 #permalink

I came across this post via genomeweb's Daily Scan, and there synesthesia (the Americanized spelling of it) was referred to as a disease. My response to this was the following:

Synesthesia is a "disease"? Since when? Frankly, I would let someone "cure" me of my synesthesia about as quickly as I'd let them "cure" me of my ability to taste sweetness or smell a rose blossom. I feel sorry for people who AREN'T synesthetic, because they don't know what they're missing.

Really, if synesthesia is a disease just because it means I can do something most of the world can't, then as far as I'm concerned, anyone who can play the piano or fix a jet engine is diseased, because I can't do either of those.

By nighthawk808 (not verified) on 06 Feb 2009 #permalink

The potential linkage of synesthesia to regulation of the reelin gene and other genes related to autism and epilepsy is extremely interesting. Reelin in particular not only regulates development of cerebral cortex but has key roles in the development and function of other regions of the brain including hippocampus and cerebellum. There is compelling evidence that Reelin can regulate the growth of neuronal processes and synapses in addition to its better known role in neuronal migration. One can imagine that interactions amongst a variety of genes could lead to localized dysregulation of reelin and other genes involved in brain development in such a way as to establish novel circuits that would allow mixing and matching of sensory modalities. If reelin is a player in synesthesia a localized change in its function might be expected since mutations that cause the global loss of reelin function in the developing brain cause Lissencephaly with Cerebellar Hypoplasia, a disastrous condition. This will be an interesting to story to follow as it develops.

I'm very glad I stumbled across this. I'll try to stumble back here more often. Thank you.

By Dave Sherry (not verified) on 08 Feb 2009 #permalink

So far as I know, I'm the only one in the nuclear family I was born into that has it, and of my three children, only one has it and we disagree violently about what color something is. I've always viewed it as yet another sign of my weirdness, but I have two friends at the moment (one an artist) who are fascinated by it. Until their fascination, I hadn't thought about it much--it's just your worldview, part of who you are, so why analyze it? But I suppose it is nice to learn at age 54 that others exist who are potentially as weird as I am.

My sister and I as children both experienced words as having colors. We used to have discussions about what colors the words were, and we differed. We never thought there was something wrong with us, and I was happy to learn in the 1970's that there was a name for this.

My mother had perfect pitch, and I am wondering if this could be a related mental phenomenon.

For me, over the years, (I am now 70) the color association has faded somewhat, but I am so happy that I have had this
"multimedia" experience. I'm only sorry that I didn't have other synesthetic effects, such as musical notes associated with words.

By Sylvia Lanza (not verified) on 06 May 2009 #permalink