Quantifying the Unconscious: New Methods in fMRI Pattern Classification

i-8df6cca3f243ab02d498057e053ff30b-fmri_image.jpgFunctional magnetic resonance imaging (fMRI) has captured the popular imagination since its introduction in the early 1990s, at least partially because of the stunningly beautiful images it generates. Although it has mostly used to identify brain regions involved in specific cognitive operations, new pattern classification techniques have been applied to fMRI data in what some have called "mind reading technology." These techniques go beyond simply showing which brain areas are more active than others during a particular task to reveal functional relationships among multiple brain areas, while simultaneously avoiding both the spatial averaging and the low signal-to-noise ratios of traditional MRI methods. Exaggerations and speculations followed this development, including some frighteningly premature attempts at using this in the Indian justice system (and recent indications that parts of the US judicial system may be interested as well).

Although some of this fanfare is well deserved, these new techniques have at least one large shortcoming from a cognitive neuroscience perspective: we cannot know which of the spatial patterns of neural activity are intrinsically related to a certain task, and which are "epiphenomenal." That is, some of the differentiating characteristics used by these algorithms might be merely correlational, and might fall apart if the methods were applied to a wider group of individuals or to stimuli with different characteristics.

Williams, Dang & Kanwisher addressed this concern by integrating information from correct and incorrect trials in a simple task. Typically, only correct trials are used in fMRI studies, due to the common assumption that there's only one right way to do a task, and many wrong ways (thus, analyzing incorrect trials might statistically clutter the processes involved in good task performance). But Williams et al. reversed this logic: if pattern classification of fMRI is to focus on the real "meaningful" spatial patterns of neural activity, those extracted patterns should be precisely those which are most related to good task performance.

i-3728c37796cad5cc8ad97f925cbd489b-kanwishercategories.jpgWilliams et al. had 6 subjects categorize abstract shapes as belonging to one of three categories - spikies, smoothies and cubies (see inset image) - while inside an fMRI scanner. The authors isolated two different brain "areas of interest" as sensitive to the differences between these categories, and subtracted the correlation between categories within a single area from that within categories in a single area, and compared this between incorrect and correct trials. This calculation basically indicates whether the activity patterns correlated due to category membership are also related to task performance. In both areas of interest (lateral occipital cortex and retinotopic occipital cortex) activity was more highly correlated within than between categories, but this correlation was stronger for correct classifications only in the lateral occipital cortex.

In other words, while retinotopic occipital cortex appears to represent information that can be used by a computer algorithm to discriminate these categories of items, only "downstream" activity in the lateral areas actually correlates with performance. So although the information needed to discriminate "spikies" from "smoothies" exists in multiple parts of the brain, only some of that appears to be used in overt responding. Indeed, even when retinotopic cortex contains patterns which are reliably related to the correct answer, subjects did not appear to use this information in their judgment!

One might say that these informative representations exist at a sub- or unconscious level, since they are not used in providing a response. Williams et al preferred more subtle language: they claim that "only some spatial patterns of fMRI response are read out in task performance." Still, the underlying claim is the same: the brain contains this information but is apparently not utilizing it in cognition.

More like this

A 2010 FINS paper from Cohen et al. demonstrates that multivariate patterns in neural recruitment during response inhibition across the brain are significantly predictive of response inhibition ability and age of the scanned subject, and shows that other factors (such as response variability and…
"Simplicity is the ultimate sophistication." - Leonardo Da Vinci "The aim of science is to seek the simplest explanation of complex facts. We are apt to fall into the error of thinking that the facts are simple because simplicity is the goal of our quest. The guiding motto in the life of every…
Two seemingly contradictory trends characterize brain development during childhood and adolescence: Diffuse to focal: a shift from relatively diffuse recruitment of neural regions to more focal and specific patterns of activity, whether in terms of the number of regions recruited, or the magnitude…
According to artificial neural network models that implement lateral inhibition, activation and inhibition are two sides of the same coin. These models often assume that patterns of activation compete with one another. In other words, in a given space of neural tissue (or "layer" in network terms…

this is very interesting...

I think there are three basic theories that people recognize objects by: template, feature and structure.

This finding is threatening to make the template theory obsolete!

0_o...

But it could be other things.

1.The objects are just too abstract to be compared to original templates of spikeys, smoothies and cubies.

2. The words spikeys, smoothies and cubies are already abstract in nature because there is no fixed structure for either one.

3. The abstract shapes are combinations of two categories. FOr example the top picture, it's more spikey, but can be a combination of a spikey and a smoothie because the round part in the middle is smooth. The correlational signals that were substracted might actually be part of the process of trying to differentiate between which category the shaped fit into more accurately. It could be that the information used is after a decision making process, which makes the person choose an answer that is available (in this case, choosing one of the categories).

Maybe this could be further determined if the participants were further asked to describe how they decided with thier answers, and then seeing which part of thier brains were more active during the organization of how they processed information to come up with an answer.

By MoonShadow (not verified) on 25 Jun 2007 #permalink